Skip to main content
Advertisement
Browse Subject Areas
?

Click through the PLOS taxonomy to find articles in your field.

For more information about PLOS Subject Areas, click here.

  • Loading metrics

Fitter Women Did Not Have Attenuated Hemodynamic Responses to Psychological Stress Compared with Age-Matched Women with Lower Levels of Fitness

  • Sisitha U. Jayasinghe,

    Current address: Biomedical Sciences, Cornell University, Ithaca, NY, United States of America

    Affiliation School of Exercise and Nutrition Science, Institute for Physical Activity and Nutrition Research, Geelong, VIC, Australia

  • Susan J. Torres,

    Affiliation School of Exercise and Nutrition Science, Institute for Physical Activity and Nutrition Research, Geelong, VIC, Australia

  • Mais Hussein,

    Affiliation School of Exercise and Nutrition Science, Institute for Physical Activity and Nutrition Research, Geelong, VIC, Australia

  • Steve F. Fraser,

    Affiliation School of Exercise and Nutrition Science, Institute for Physical Activity and Nutrition Research, Geelong, VIC, Australia

  • Gavin W. Lambert,

    Current address: Central Clinical School, Monash University, Melbourne, VIC, Australia

    Affiliation Baker IDI Heart & Diabetes Institute, Melbourne, VIC, Australia

  • Anne I. Turner

    anne.turner@deakin.edu.au

    Affiliation School of Exercise and Nutrition Science, Institute for Physical Activity and Nutrition Research, Geelong, VIC, Australia

Abstract

According to the ‘cross stressor adaptation hypothesis’, regular exercise acts as a buffer against the detrimental effects of stress. Nevertheless, evidence that higher levels of cardiorespiratory fitness moderate hemodynamic responses to acute psychological stress is inconclusive, especially in women. Women aged 30–50 years (in the mid-follicular phase of the menstrual cycle) with higher (n = 17) and lower (n = 17) levels of fitness were subjected to a Trier Social Stress Test (TSST). Continuous, non-invasive measurements were made of beat-to-beat, systolic blood pressure (SBP), diastolic blood pressure (DBP), mean arterial pressure (MAP), heart rate (HR), stroke volume (SV), cardiac output (CO), left ventricular ejection time (LVET), maximum slope, pulse interval (PI) and total peripheral resistance (TPR). Maximal oxygen consumption was significantly (p<0.001) higher in the ‘higher fit’ women. Lower fit women had higher fasting glucose, resting heart rate, waist to hip ratios and elevated serum triglyceride and cholesterol/ HDL ratios compared with higher fit women (p<0.05 for all). While all measured parameters (for both groups)displayed significant (p<0.001) responses to the TSST, only HR, PI and LVET differed significantly between higher and lower fit women (p<0.001 for all) with the higher fit women having the larger response in each case. It was also found that higher fit women had significantly shorter time to recovery for maximum slope compared with the lower fit women. These findings provide little support for the notion that higher levels of cardiorespiratory fitness result in lower cardiovascular responsivity to psychological stress in women but may indicate that lower fit women have blunted responses to stress.

Introduction

Exposure to psychological stress can rapidly activate the autonomic nervous system wherethe activation of the sympathetic nervous system (SNS) can result in numerous cardiovascular responsesincluding a rise in heart rate, blood pressure, cardiac output and total peripheral resistance [1]. Physiological responses to psychological stressors are generated due to anticipation of a possible disruption of physiological homeostasis, rather than to a genuine threat. Therefore, the stress response generated in such instances is “anticipatory” [2]. While it is essential to sufficiently activate the SNS in response to stress (to facilitate an adaptive response to the stressor encountered), unnecessary over activation of the SNS can be detrimental and may lead to chronic diseases including cardiovascular disease, type 2 diabetes and psychological disorders [38].

The cross stressor adaptation hypothesis states that ‘A stressor of sufficient intensity and/or duration will induce an adaptation of the stress response systems, which becomes apparent in other similarly taxing states’[9]. In agreement with this hypothesis,higher levels of cardiorespiratory fitness achieved via physical exercise, acts as a buffer against the detrimental effects of stress [1012]. Exercise, upon repetition is thought to elicit counterconditioning to other stimuli perhaps through mechanisms such as the central catecholamine system and the central/peripheral opioid system [13]. The general consensus is that regular exercise that increases cardiorespiratory fitness is associated with an attenuation of cardiovascular responses to a wide variety of stressors[9, 10, 14, 15]. Nevertheless, the evidence regarding cardiorespiratory fitness and cardiovascular system reactivity in response to psychological stress remains inconclusive, especially in women. For instance, there are studies that have found both beneficial effects of fitness (i.e. reduction in heart rate and blood pressure responses to stress) [1621], as well as no influence of fitness on cardiovascular responses to stress[2224]. In a recent study conducted by our research group, we showed that increased cardiorespiratory fitness did not moderate cortisol, catecholamine or cardiovascular responses to psychological stress (Trier Social Stress Test; TSST) in 30–50 year old women who had varying levels of physical fitness[25]. However, cardiovascular parameters were only measured every 7–15 minutes so it is possible that some outcomes were not detected. It is also important to acknowledge that there is an increasing body of evidence that suggests that blunted cardiovascular and endocrine responses to acute stress may also be associated with ill health [26]. By recruiting seemingly healthy women we expect to have minimised instances of a ‘blunted’ stress response from this study. Nevertheless, reduced responses to stress may be indicative of poorer health outcomes.

The inconclusive results in previous experiments may be due to variations in methodologies that have been implemented. A wide variety of stressors such as mental arithmetic with auditory distraction, competitive motor tasks and competitive reaction time tasks [18, 27, 28]were used in previous experiments whichmay not have been capable of eliciting a robust and repeatable autonomic nervous system response as occurs when using the TSST[2932]. In addition, previous studies lack continuous measuremen tof cardiovascular parameters. The rapidly changing nature of the cardiovascular response necessitates frequent sampling to capture the finer details ofthe response patterns. This may also provide a more holistic view of the cardiovascular responses to stress. To the best of our knowledge, a complete hemodynamic profile has not been measured in experiments that have investigated the effects of physical activity levels on cardiovascular responses to psychological stress. In addition, fewer studies have investigated these responses in women[14]. Given the known sex differences in stress responsiveness [33], findings in males are not necessarily generalisableto females.

The aim of this study was to investigate the impact of cardiorespiratory fitness onhemodynamic responses to a TSST using continuous non-invasive sampling in 30–50 year old women. A complete profile of hemodynamic responses was assessed through the measurement of systolic blood pressure (SBP), diastolic blood pressure (DBP), mean arterial pressure (MAP), heart rate (HR), stroke volume (SV), cardiac output (CO), left ventricular ejection time (LVET), maximum slope, pulse interval (PI) and total peripheral resistance (TPR). It was hypothesised that women who have higher levels of cardiorespiratory fitness will have lower hemodynamic cardiovascular responsivity compared with age matched women who have relatively lower levels of cardiorespiratory fitness.

Materials and Methods

Healthy women with no history or current metabolic disease (n = 34;aged 30–50 years) were recruited from the Melbourne metropolitan area, Victoria, Australia using several methods including news paper and online advertisements, emails, fliers in community centres and medical clinics. Women were excluded if they had current or prior diagnosis/incidence of: Cushing’s syndrome, any stress or anxiety disorder, depression, any diseases of the adrenal gland, type 2 diabetes, heart disease, elevated total cholesterol (>5.5 mmol/L), stroke, cancer (including cancer remission) or any other disorder that may potentially impact the stress pathway activity. None of the women were medicated at the time of recruitment or at the time of either testing session. Given the influence of sex steroids on stress pathway activity, women were also excluded if they were post-menopausal, peri-menopausal or were on any form of steroidal contraception (including oral contraceptives, steroidal implants and steroidal intrauterine devices) [33].

All procedures implemented in this research were approved by the Human Research Ethics Committee of Deakin University (Project code: 2011–242) and conformed to the guidelines of the National Health and Medical Research Council’s National Statement on Ethical Conduct in Human Research (2007). Participants were also required to provide informed consent prior to commencing the study.

Experimental procedure

All testing was conducted at our Clinical Research Facility in the School of Exercise and Nutrition Sciences at Deakin University. Each participant attended our facility on two separate testing days. Details of the procedures undertaken on each testing day are included below. The second testing day occurred at least one week after the first.

Day 1 testing

A fasting blood sample for the measurement of cardio-metabolic risk markers was collected and maximum oxygen consumption (VO2max) was determined. Participants were given instructions to fast overnight (for at least 10 hours) prior to attending the laboratory. Body weight (TANITA, Wedderburn, Melbourne, Australia), height (Measurement Concepts, North Bend, Australia), BMI (weight/ (height in meters)2) and resting blood pressure (Criticare systems Inc, Wisconsin, USA) were recorded. Women whose BMI fell outside the range 18–30 (kg/m2) or whose resting blood pressure exceeded 140mmHg for SBP or 90 mmHg for DBP were excluded. A blood sample (GreinerBio-One GmbH, Kremsmunster, Austria) was collected from all eligible women through standard venepuncture procedures for the analysis of cardiometabolic risk markers (total cholesterol, high density lipoprotein, low density lipoprotein, triglycerides, fasting serum glucose, C-reactive protein and fasting insulin).

The women were then offered an optional snack (a selection of foods from muesli bars, nuts, dried fruit and juice boxes were made available) and further gathering of information was undertaken via a Physical Activity Readiness Questionnaire (PAR-Q), an International Physical Activity Questionnaire (IPAQ) [34], a State-Trait Anxiety Inventory (STAI) [35] and a Beck Depression Inventory (BDI-ii) [36]. Information from the PAR-Q (confirming suitability to undertake vigorous intensity exercise) was used as an eligibility criterion priorto undertakinga VO2max test. A thorough explanation of the requirements of the fitness test was provided to all participants after the snack. This was followed by a graded VO2maxtest according to the methods that have been previously used by our research group [25]. After ranking women by VO2max score, a median split was used to allocate women evenly into a group with high VO2max (higher fit group; n = 17) and a group with low VO2max (lower fit group; n = 17) [37]. Water was available ad libitum to all participants throughout the testing session.

Day 2 testing

To standardise the influence of stage of menstrual cycle on results, this session was conducted in the mid follicular phase (Days 5–9) of the participants’ menstrual cycle[38]. This time frame for the mid follicular phase is consistent with multiple previous reports that investigated the relationship between phase of the menstrual cycle and the activity of the stress pathways [3942]. All womenwere asked to abstain from smoking, ingesting any caffeine containing beverages (e.g. tea, coffee, cola), liquorice, alcohol or drugs (except for any regular medications) and from strenuous physical activity during the 12 hours prior to participation in the Stress testing session.

The experimental procedure for Day 2 is shown in Fig 1. Participants arrived at the laboratory at 1100h. Anthropometric measures (waist and hip circumference andbio impedance estimation of body fat [TANITA, Wedderburn, Melbourne, Australia]) were obtained between 1100h-1145h. Waist circumference was measured at the midpoint between the last rib and the anterior superior iliac spine, using a tape measure, and hip circumferencewas measured at the widest point of the gluteal area[43]. A questionnaire regarding smoking status, alcohol consumption and physical activity in the week leading up to the testing sessionwas completed by the participants during this time. Astandard lunch containing approximately 20% protein, 61% carbohydrate and 19% fat was consumed by all participants between 1200h-1230h. As previously reported by our group there were no differences in the cortisol, blood pressure and heart rate responses in higher and lower fit women in response to the standardised lunch [44]. This was followed by the sampling procedures which began at 1230h. Continuous beat-to-beat SBP, DBP, HR,CO, TPR, LVET, SV, PI and maximum slope were measured using aFinometer (Finometer model-1 (PRO); Finopres Medical Systems BV, Amsterdam, the Netherlands) attached to a finger on the non-dominant arm. This unit also contained sensors for the adjustment of movement artefacts.A period of familiarisation with the sampling procedures took place between 1230h-1330h. Participants were given a break at 1330h in which they made avisit to the restroom. Further beat-to-beat monitoring was undertaken from 1400h to 1700h. A TSST was imposed between 1500h-1530h (details below) and a recovery period was monitoredfrom 1530h-1700h.

thumbnail
Fig 1. Schematic representation of the stress testing day.

TSST = Trier Social Stress Test, B = biological samples (results from biological samples are reported elsewhere [25]).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0169746.g001

TSST

Psychological stress was induced using a modified version [45] of the well characterised Trier Social Stress Test (TSST) protocol[29]. The modifications included a 2 personnel (instead of 3) interviewing panel, a 60-minute lead in time prior to exposure to stress and biological sampling in-between the public speaking task and the mental arithmetic challenge. At 1500h, participants were introduced to a panel and were given instructions about a speaking task to follow (Fig 1, inset). This was followed by a 10min preparation phase and then five minutes of public speaking. Further instructions followed regarding a mental arithmetic task and then 5 min of mental arithmetic was undertaken. Continuous cardiovascular parameter measurements using the Finometer were made throughout this entire period.

Statistical analysis

Preliminary analysis.

Beat-to-beat monitoring of cardiovascular information from 1400h-1700h resulted in approximately 15000–16000 data points for each subject. For clarity of data presentation, means of 2 minute blocks were calculated for the entire sampling period. Data from 1400–1430h were excluded from the final analysis due to complications (such as missing data) during the initial 30 minutes of sampling. This resulted in 15, 16 and 44 time points for pre-treatment, stress and recovery phases, respectively.

Pre-treatment for all measured parameters was defined as the average of means of the 15, 2-minute blocks from 1430h-1500h. Peak height for blood pressures, HR and CO was defined as the highest value obtained between 1500h-1600h inclusive. Since some cardiovascular parameters (LVET, SV and pulse interval) decrease rather than increase in response to acute stress, lowest trough for LVET, SV and pulse interval was defined as the lowest reading recorded between 1500h-1600h inclusive. This variable is the equivalent of peak height but is used for parameters that decrease instead of increasing in response to acute stress. Reactivity was calculated as the difference between pre-treatment and peak height/ lowest trough for all parameters. Area under the curve (with respect to increase) was calculated for each parameter using values from 1500 to 1700 h after the subtraction of the pre-treatment value from each data point[46]. Areas under the curve were calculated using the trapezoid rule utilising Sigmaplot graphing software (SystatSoftwareInc., California, USA).

As described by our group previously [45], recovery time for all parameters was defined as the time difference from the commencement of the stressor (1500h) to the point at which the relevant parameter returned to within two standard deviations of its pre-treatment value. Those who did not have a significant response to the stressor (i.e. those who did not exceed two standard deviations of the pre-treatment value between 1500–1700h; n = 1 [PI] lower fit, n = 1, 1 [LVET] lower/higher fit, n = 1, 1 [SV] lower/higher fit, n = 5, 1 [CO] lower/higher fit, n = 3 [TPR] lower fit, n = 1 [max slope] lower fit) were excluded from analyses of recovery time. For those who did exceed two standard deviations but did not return to within two standard deviations by 1700h (n = 1, 1 [SBP] lower/higher fit, n = 2, 2 [DBP] lower/higher fit, n = 1, 2 [MAP] lower/higher fit, n = 1 [SV] lower fit, n = 2, 3 [TPR] lower/higher fit, n = 1 [max slope] lower fit), 120min was used as the recovery time in the analyses.

Analysis.

Data were analysed using the Statistical Package for the Social Sciences software version 20.0 (SPSS. Inc, Chicago, USA). Kolmogorov-Smirnov and Shapiro–Wilk tests were conducted to test for normality. Tests for homogeneity of variance were conducted using Levene’s test of equality of error variances. Descriptive characteristics were compared between groups using univariate analysis of variance. Cardiovascular parameters were compared within and between groups using repeated measures analysis of variance. The within subjects factor was time and the between subjects factor was cardiorespiratory fitness (higher fit vs lower fit). Derived cardiovascular parameters (pre- treatment, peak height/lowest trough, reactivity, area under the curve and recovery time) were compared between groups using univariate analysis of variance. Pearson’s correlation without adjustment and with adjustments (partial correlations) for central adiposity (by means of WHR) and metabolic parameters (by means of HOMA-IR) were conducted between VO2 max and reactivity and area under the curve of all dependant variables.

Results

Participants

Data from 34 women are presented in this study. The data presented in this paper are part of a larger study (17) which assessed cortisol and catecholamine responses to psychological stress in women. A total of 44 women were tested in the larger study. Nevertheless, ten women had to be excluded from the current analysis due to incomplete data.The remaining 34 women,used in this study,were ranked by VO2 max and a median split was used to allocate women to either the lower fit (n = 17) or the higher fit group (n = 17). The 10 women who were excluded from the larger study did not differ from the final cohort in any of the descriptive characteristics (data not shown). Trait anxiety score, insulin data and HOMA-IR were unavailable for two women (both from the higher fit group).

Participant characteristics

VO2 max was significantly higher in women in the higher fit group compared with the women in the lower fit group (Table 1). As expected, VO2 max and the number of hours of physical activity per week were significantly positively correlated (r = 0.428; p = 0.012). Both groups were in the healthy weight range and had similar BMI levels, body fat percentages, waist circumference and hip circumference (Table 1). However, WHR was significantly higher (p = 0.041) in the lower fit women compared with the higher fit women (Table 1). Resting heart rate (p = 0.048) and diastolic blood pressure (p = 0.004) were also higher in the lower fit women compared with the higher fit women (Table 1). Resting SBP, resting SBP variability and resting DBP variability were comparable between low and high fit women (Table 1). Triglycerides (p = 0.034), fasting glucose (p = 0.045) and CHOL/HDL ratio (p = 0.005) were significantly higher in the lower fit women compared with the higher fit women (Table 2). All other cardio metabolic risk factors and depression/anxiety scores were comparable between the groups (Table 2).

thumbnail
Table 1. Mean (±SEM) descriptive characteristics of Lower and Higher fit women.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0169746.t001

thumbnail
Table 2. Mean (±SEM) cardio-metabolic risk markers and depression and anxiety scores of Lower and Higher fit women.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0169746.t002

HR, PI and LVET

HR, PIand LVET in lower fit and higher fit women are shown in Fig 2A, 2B and 2C, respectively and Table 3. Repeated measures analysis of variance revealed that there was a significant effect of time for all three parameters (p<0.001 for all; Fig 2A, 2B and 2C). Overall (both groups combined), the peak height of HR (93±3 bpm) and lowest troughs of PI (680±20 ms) and LVET (262±4 ms) were significantly higher/lower than their respective pre-treatment values (69±2 bpm, 899±22 ms and 306±3 ms) (p<0.001). Overall, there was a 35% increase in HR, an11% decrease in PI and a14% decrease in LVET from pre-treatment values to the highest peak/lowest trough of the response (both groups combined).

thumbnail
Fig 2.

Mean (± SEM) heart rate (2a), pulse interval (2b) and left ventricular ejection time (2c) in lower and higher fit women from 1430h-1700h. TSST: Trier Social Stress Test.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0169746.g002

thumbnail
Table 3. Mean (±SEM) pre-treatment, peak height/lowest trough, reactivity, area under the curve and recovery time for HR, LVET and Pulse interval in lower and higher fit women.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0169746.t003

In response to the TSST, HR, PI and LVET all differed between lower fit women and higher fit women (time*treatment, p<0.001 for all; Fig 2A, 2B and 2C).In keeping with this finding, reactivity and AUC for HR and PI were significantly larger in higher fit women than in lower fit women (p≤0.05 for all; Table 3). Nevertheless, reactivity and AUC for LVET did not differ significantly between the groups (Table 3).There were no significant differences between groups in peak height/lowest trough or time to recovery in any of the three parameters (Table 3). Significant between subjects effects in HR, PI and LVET were evident for all three variables (p< 0.05 for all)indicating that there were significant overall differences between the groups. This was reflected in pre-treatment values for HR being significantly lower in higher fit women and in pre-treatment values for PI and LVET being significantly higher in higher fit women (p<0.05 for all; Fig 2).

SBP, DBP and MAP

All blood pressure parameters in lower fit and higher fit women are shown in Fig 3. Repeated measures analysis of variance revealed that there was a significant effect of time for all blood pressure parameters (p<0.001 for all; Fig 3). Overall (both groups combined), the peak heights of SBP (165±5 mmHg), DBP (94±2 mmHg) and MAP (121±3 mmHg) were significantly higher than their respective pre-treatment values (119±3, 68±1 and 88±2 mmHg;p<0.001 for all). Overall, there was a 39% increase in SBP, a 38% increase in DBP and a 36% increase in MAP from pre-treatment pressures to the peak of the response (both groups combined).

thumbnail
Fig 3. Mean (± SEM) SBP, DBP and MAP in lower and higher fit women from 1430h-1700h.

TSST: Trier Social Stress Test.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0169746.g003

None of the blood pressure parameters in response to the TSST differed between lower fit women and higher fit women (time*treatment, p = 0.854 (SBP), p = 0.997 (DBP) and p = 0.996 (MAP); Fig 3) and accordingly, there were no significant differences between the groups in peak height, reactivity, area under the curve or mean time to recovery for the blood pressure responses (data not shown). A significant between subjects effect indicated that overall, SBP, DBP and MAP were significantly higher (p = 0.025, 0.040 and 0.020 for SBP, DBP and MAP; respectively) in the lower fit women compared with higher fit women(Fig 3). This was reflected in pre-treatment values for all parameters which were significantly higher (p = 0.019 [SBP], p = 0.054 [DBP] and p = 0.014 [MAP]) in the lower fit women compared with thehigher fit women (125±4 vs 113±3 [SBP], 71±2 vs 66±2 [DBP] and 92±3 vs 84±2 [MAP] mmHg, respectively).

SV, CO, TPR and maximum slope

SV, CO, TPR and maximum slope in lower fit and higher fit women are shown in Fig 4A, 4B, 4C and 4D, respectively. Repeated measures analysis of variance revealed that there was a significant effect of time for all of the aforementioned parameters (p<0.001 for all; Fig 4). Overall (both groups combined), the lowest trough of SV (55±2 mL) and the peak height of CO (7±0.3L), TPR (2499±173dyn·s/cm5) and max slope (2079±155 mmHg/s) were significantly lower/higher than their respective pre-treatment values (79±3 mL, 5±0.2 L, 1407±45 dyn·s/cm5and 1129±52 mmHg/s) (p<0.001). Overall, there was a 29% decrease in SV, a 32% increase in CO, a 77% increase in TPR and an84% increase in max slope from pre-treatment values to the lowest trough / highest peak of the response (both groups combined).

thumbnail
Fig 4.

Mean (± SEM) stroke volume (4a), cardiac output (4b), total peripheral resistance (4c) and maximum slope in lower and higher fit women from 1430h-1700h. TSST: Trier Social Stress Test.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0169746.g004

None of these parameters differed between the lower fit women and higher fit women in response to the TSST(time*treatment, p = 0.956 (SV), p = 0.953 (CO), p = 0.791 (TPR) and p = 0.609 (max slope); Fig 4) and accordingly, there were no significant differences between the groups in peak height/lowest trough, reactivity or area under the curve for any of these parameters (data not shown). The mean time to recovery was comparable between the groups for SV, CO and TPR, but mean time to recovery for max slope was significantly longer(p = 0.048) in the lower fit women compared with the higher fit women (41±6 and 28±2 min, respectively). There was no significant between subjects effect in SV, CO or TPR indicating that there were no significant overall differences between the groups for any of these variables (p = 0.512, 0.332 and 0.500 for SV, CO and TPR; respectively). Nevertheless, there was a significant overall difference (p = 0.016) between the groups in max slope (Fig 4D) with lower fit women having a significantly higher max slope overall than higher fit women. This was reflected in there being a significantly higher (p = 0.008) max slope during pre-treatment in the lower fit women compared with the higher fit women (1262±82 and 995±49 mmHg/s, respectively).

Correlations/ Associations

In unadjusted correlations, there were significant (P<0.05) associations between VO2max and the reactivity and AUC for heart rate and pulse interval but no associations with any of the other cardiovascular parameters (Table 4). When adjustment was made for central adiposity (by means of WHR) and metabolic parameters (by means of HOMA-IR), a significant association was seen between VO2max and the reactivity and AUC for mean arterial pressure and between VO2max and the AUC for cardiac output (Table 4),

thumbnail
Table 4. Associations between VO2max and reactivity and area under the curve (AUC) for cardiovascular parameters.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0169746.t004

Discussion

This study found that women who had higher levels of cardiorespiratory fitness (higher fit women) had greater HR, LVET and PI responses to a potent psychological stress compared with aged-matched women who had lower levels of cardiorespiratory fitness (lower fit women).It was also found that higher fit women had a significantly shorter time to recovery for maximum slope compared with the lower fit women. Furthermore, after adjustment for central adiposity and metabolic parameters, the response of mean arterial pressure (reactivity and AUC) and cardiac output (AUC) was found to be positively associated with VO2max indicating that higher fit women had higher responses of these parameters when controlling for covariates. All other parameters (SBP, DBP, SV and TPR) were comparable between the groups in response to the TSST. This suggests that cardiovascular and hemodynamic responses to psychological stress are only partially influenced by the level of cardiorespiratory fitness. While the current results mostly suggest an association between higher levels of cardiorespiratory fitness and heightened activity of the cardiovascular system (indicated by greater HR, LVET, PI, MAP and CO) in response to psychological stress, the rapid recovery of maximum slope in higher fit women suggests there may be some improvement of response in the higher fit women albeit a very subtle effect. These findings provide little support for the notion that women with higher levels of cardiorespiratory fitness will have lower cardiovascular responsivity to psychological stress compared with women who have relatively lower levels of cardiorespiratory fitness. Nevertheless, these findings may indicate that lower fit women have reduced or blunted HR, LVET, PI, MAP and CO responses to stress compared with high fit women.

Both higher and lower fit women reached similar HR peaks in response to the TSST. These peaks are well within the range of HR responses to TSSTs reported before [47]. However, it is evident in the current study that the higher fit women had a lower HR before the commencement of the stress testdespite peaking at the same level as the lower fit women during exposure to psychological stress. As such, there may be a “ceiling” effect with HR which prevents it from further elevations in either group. This provides the appearance of an accentuated HR response in higher fit women. Nevertheless, this could merely be a result of the higher fit women having a lower baseline thus overall, still having the healthier response.A similar response was observed in one of our previous studies in lean and overweight/obese men [1]. In that study, lean men had a lower HR at the commencement of the TSST, but reached a maximum that was comparable to the overweight/obese men after exposure to stress.However, the findings in the present study where the HR reactivity was greater in the higher fit women compared to lower fit women disagree with other published findings. Traustadottir et al. 2005 reported greater HR reactivity to a Matt Stress Reactivity Protocol in young unfit individuals compared with older fit and unfit women. Higher HR responses were also reported in inactive young women in comparison to their more active counterparts [48] and more recent findings in men show lower HR responses to a TSST after improving fitness through exercise intervention [21]. While it is not consistent with the results of the studies listed above, an alternative way of considering our data may be that the fitter women are having a normal response to the stressor while lower fit women are having a reduced or blunted response. As indicated earlier, reduced responsivity of the stress systems may also be associated with ill health [26].

Our finding that the response of mean arterial pressure (reactivity and AUC) and cardiac output (AUC) were significantly positively associated with VO2max after adjustment for central adiposity and metabolic parameters may indicate a similar circumstance to that of heart rate. With fitter women starting from a lower baseline for these variables, it is possible that they have reached a similar “ceiling” of response as the lower fit women. Nevertheless, these findings are not consistent with those of Palatini and colleagues who showed a higher blood pressure response in sedentary hypertensive individuals in response to a public speaking task compared with both active hypertensive individuals and controls [49]. As with heart rate, an alternative may be that the lower fit women are displaying a blunted response for mean arterial pressure and cardiac output.

The successful execution of a stress experiment depends on the ability of the stressor utilised to elicit a substantial reaction from the physiological stress pathways that are being investigated. In the current study, all measured parameters showed significant changes in response to the TSST. The various components of the TSST have shown to generate substantial cardiovascular responses consistently in comparison to executing a solitary stressor [50]. However, it is possible that our modified version of the TSST used in the current experiment may have maximally acutely activated the cardiovascular system in both groups. As such, it is possible that, had a more subtle (sub-maximal) stressor been used, the lower fit women may indeed have shown a larger response compared with the higher fit women.

Alternatively, it is also possible that the pattern of results observed in the current study is an indication that cardiorespiratory fitness in females may bare less impact on reducing excessive cardiovascular responses to psychological stresscompared with men. Previous reports by Rimmele and colleagues have shown that in men, elevated levels of cardiorespiratory fitness are associated with attenuated cardiovascular responses (lower HR) to psychological stress[16, 17]. We did not observe an obvious attenuation of cardiovascular reactivity pattern in the higher-fit women in the current experiment. Therefore, it could be speculated that attenuation in cardiovascular reactivity in response to psychological stress mediated by fitness status is influenced by sex and sex steroids.

As mentioned in the introduction[26], there is evidence that supports the notion of a blunted physiological response to acute stress being associated with symptoms related to ill health including, but not limited to, obesity[51], depression[52] and poorer cognitive function [53]. Reasons for a blunted reaction to acute stimuli may include reduced physiological capacity to respond to a given stimulus and a variety of psychological reasons[26].All women who participated in the current experiment were seemingly healthy, and all of them were capable of eliciting a substantial response to the TSST. Furthermore, based on the depression and anxiety levels (judged by the BDI-11 and STAI), there is no grounds to speculate that the women included in this study were psychologically compromised. Nevertheless, it is possible that the lower level of response seen in the lower fit women was indicative of a reduced ability to mount a normal stress response. Hypo-responsivity of the stress response such as this may be associated with poorer health outcomes [26].

Aside from the responses to psychosocial stress in lower and higher fit women, it should be noted that the lower fit women had a poorer cardiovascular profile overall than higher fit women. Overall, lower fit women had higher HR, SBP, DBP, MAP and maximum slope and lower PI and LVET compared with higher fit women. When you consider that these women also had higher waste to hip ratio, triglycerides, CHOL/HDL ratio and fasting glucose than higher fit women, it is clear that the overall health profile of these women was reduced compared with that of the higher fit women.

A significant strength of the current experiment is the continuous measurement of beat to beat cardiovascular changes. This measurement strategy, which has been validated for the use in cardiovascular research [54, 55],enables precise detection of cardiovascular changes and provides a more holistic view of the stress response [56]. We also utilised a modified version of the TSST which elicited substantial cardiovascular reactions in both groups of women. Future research could include women with much larger differences in fitness levels and women who are in different phases of their menstrual cycles. It is also noteworthy that the fitness levels of the women who took part in this study were representative of the prevailing fitness norms of 30–50 year old women [57]. Nevertheless, the authors acknowledge that exercise dependence was neither measured nor controlled for in the current study. Such measurements were beyond the scope of this study.

According to the ‘cross stressor adaptation hypothesis’, regular physical exercise acts as a buffer against the detrimental effects of stress. We tested the hypothesis that women who have higher levels of cardiorespiratory fitness will have lower cardiovascular responsivity compared with age matched women who have relatively lower levels of cardiorespiratory fitness. Nevertheless, results reported and discussed in this paper provide little support for the notion that higher levels of cardiorespiratory fitness result in lower cardiovascular responsivity to psychological stress in women. Instead, lower fit women may have had a blunted response to stress. More investigations need to be conducted to elucidate the exact workings behind the relationship between cardiorespiratory fitness and cardiovascular responses to stress.

Supporting Information

Acknowledgments

We thank Gavin Abbott for his assistance with statistical analysis. We also thank the volunteers for their participation and the Deakin laboratory staff for their assistance.

Author Contributions

  1. Conceptualization: SJ ST MH SF GL AT.
  2. Formal analysis: SJ ST MH SF GL AT.
  3. Investigation: SJ GL AT.
  4. Methodology: SJ ST MH AT.
  5. Project administration: SJ ST MH AT.
  6. Resources: SJ ST MH AT.
  7. Validation: SJ ST MH SF GL AT.
  8. Writing – review & editing: SJ ST MH SF GL AT.

References

  1. 1. Torres SJ, Turner AI, Jayasinghe SU, Reynolds J, Nowson CA. The Effect of Overweight/Obesity on Cardiovascular Responses to Acute Psychological Stress in Men Aged 50–70 Years. Obesity facts. 2014;7(6):339–50. pmid:25428119
  2. 2. Herman JP, Figueiredo H, Mueller NK, Ulrich-Lai Y, Ostrander MM, Choi DC, et al. Central mechanisms of stress integration: Hierarchical circuitry controlling hypothalamo-pituitary-adrenocortical responsiveness. Front Neuroendocrinol. 2003;24(3):151–80. pmid:14596810
  3. 3. Chrousos GP. Stress and disorders of the stress system. Nature Reviews Endocrinology. 2009;5(7):374–81. pmid:19488073
  4. 4. Miller GE, Rohleder N, Stetler C, Kirschbaum C. Clinical depression and regulation of the inflammatory response during acute stress. Psychosom Med. 2005;67(5):679–87. pmid:16204423
  5. 5. Carroll D, Phillips AC, Der G, Hunt K, Benzeval M. Blood Pressure Reactions to Acute Mental Stress and Future Blood Pressure Status: Data From the 12-Year Follow-Up of the West of Scotland Study. Psychosom Med. 2011;73(9):737–42. pmid:22021460
  6. 6. Matthews KA, Katholi CR, McCreath H, Whooley MA, Williams DR, Zhu S, et al. Blood pressure reactivity to psychological stress predicts hypertension in the CARDIA study. Circulation. 2004;110(1):74–8. pmid:15210592
  7. 7. Esler M, Jennings G, Korner P, Willett I, Dudley F, Hasking G, et al. Assessment of human sympathetic nervous system activity from measurements of norepinephrine turnover. Hypertension. 1988;11(1):3–20. pmid:2828236
  8. 8. Lambert GW, Kaye DM, Lefkovits J, Jennings GL, Turner AG, Cox HS, et al. Increased central nervous system monoamine neurotransmitter turnover and its association with sympathetic nervous activity in treated heart failure patients. Circulation. 1995;92(7):1813–8. pmid:7545554
  9. 9. Sothmann MS, Buckworth J, Claytor RP, Cox RH, White-Welkley JE, Dishman RK. Exercise training and the cross-stressor adaptation hypothesis. Exerc Sport Sci Rev. 1996;24:267–87. pmid:8744253
  10. 10. Gerber M, Pühse U. Review article: do exercise and fitness protect against stress-induced health complaints? A review of the literature. Scand J Public Health. 2009;37(8):801–19. pmid:19828772
  11. 11. Tsatsoulis A, Fountoulakis S. The protective role of exercise on stress system dysregulation and comorbidities. In: Chrousos GP, Tsigos C, editors. Stress, Obesity, and Metabolic Syndrome. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences. 10832006. p. 196–213.
  12. 12. Hamer M, Taylor A, Steptoe A. The effect of acute aerobic exercise on stress related blood pressure responses: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Biol Psychol. 2006;71(2):183–90. pmid:15979232
  13. 13. Salmon P. Effects of physical exercise on anxiety, depression, and sensitivity to stress: a unifying theory. Clin Psychol Rev. 2001;21(1):33–61. pmid:11148895
  14. 14. Jackson EM, Dishman RK. Cardiorespiratory fitness and laboratory stress: A meta-regression analysis. Psychophysiology. 2006;43(1):57–72. pmid:16629686
  15. 15. Forcier K, Stroud LR, Papandonatos GD. Links between physical fitness and cardiovascular reactivity and recovery to psychological stressors: A meta-analysis. Health Psychol. 2006;25(6):723–39. pmid:17100501
  16. 16. Rimmele U, Zellweger BC, Marti B, Seiler R, Mohiyeddini C, Ehlert U, et al. Trained men show lower cortisol, heart rate and psychological responses to psychosocial stress compared with untrained men. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2007;32(6):627–35. pmid:17560731
  17. 17. Rimmele U, Seiler R, Marti B, Wirtz PH, Ehlert U, Heinrichs M. The level of physical activity affects adrenal and cardiovascular reactivity to psychosocial stress. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2009;34(2):190–8. pmid:18922645
  18. 18. Spalding TW, Lyon LA, Steel DH, Hatfield BD. Aerobic exercise training and cardiovascular reactivity to psychological stress in sedentary young normotensive men and women. Psychophysiology. 2004;41(4):552–62. pmid:15189478
  19. 19. Blumenthal JA, Fredrikson M, Kuhn CM, Ulmer RL, Walshriddle M, Appelbaum M. Aerobic exercise reduces levels of cardiovascular and sympathoadrenal responses to mental stress in subjects without prior evidence of myocardial ischemia. Am J Cardiol. 1990;65(1):93–8. pmid:2294687
  20. 20. Throne LC, Bartholomew JB, Craig J, Farrar RP. Stress reactivity in fire fighters: An exercise intervention. Int J Stress Manage. 2000;7(4):235–46.
  21. 21. Klaperski S, von Dawans B, Heinrichs M, Fuchs R. Effects of a 12-week endurance training program on the physiological response to psychosocial stress in men: a randomized controlled trial. J Behav Med. 2014;37(6):1118–33. pmid:24659155
  22. 22. Albright CL, King AC, Taylor CB, Haskell WL. Effect of a six-month aerobic exercise training program on cardiovascular responsivity in healthy middle-aged adults. J Psychosom Res. 1992;36(1):25–36. pmid:1538348
  23. 23. de Geus EJ, Van Doornen LJ, de Visser DC, Orlebeke JF. Existing and training induced differences in aerobic fitness: Their relationship to physiological response patterns during different types of stress. Psychophysiology. 1990.
  24. 24. Steptoe A, Moses J, Mathews A, Edwards S. Aerobic fitness, physical activity, and psychophysiological reactions to mental tasks. Psychophysiology. 1990;27(3):264–74. pmid:2236430
  25. 25. Jayasinghe SU, Lambert GW, Torres SJ, Fraser SF, Eikelis N, Turner AI. Hypothalamo-pituitary adrenal axis and sympatho-adrenal medullary system responses to psychological stress were not attenuated in women with elevated physical fitness levels. Endocrine. 2015:1–11.
  26. 26. Phillips AC, Ginty AT, Hughes BM. The other side of the coin: Blunted cardiovascular and cortisol reactivity are associated with negative health outcomes. Int J Psychophysiol. 2013;90(1):1–7. pmid:23454029
  27. 27. Anshel MH. Effect of chronic aerobic exercise and progressive relaxation on motor performance and affect following acute stress. Behav Med. 1996;21(4):186–96. pmid:8731496
  28. 28. Sherwood A, Light KC, Blumenthal JA. Effects of aerobic exercise training on hemodynamic responses during psychosocial stress in normotensive and borderline hypertensive type A men: a preliminary report. Psychosom Med. 1989;51(2):123–36. pmid:2710907
  29. 29. Kirschbaum C, Pirke KM, Hellhammer DH. The Trier Social stress test—A tool for investigating psychobiological stress responses in a laboratory setting. Neuropsychobiology. 1993;28(1–2):76–81.
  30. 30. Kudielka BM, Schommer NC, Hellhammer DH, Kirschbaum C. Acute HPA axis responses, heart rate, and mood changes to psychosocial stress (TSST) in humans at different times of day. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2004;29(8):983–92. pmid:15219648
  31. 31. Schommer NC, Hellhammer DH, Kirschbaum C. Dissociation between reactivity of the hypothalamus-pituitary-adrenal axis and the sympathetic-adrenal-medullary system to repeated psychosocial stress. Psychosom Med. 2003;65(3):450–60. pmid:12764219
  32. 32. Skoluda N, Strahler J, Schlotz W, Niederberger L, Marques S, Fischer S, et al. Intra-individual psychological and physiological responses to acute laboratory stressors of different intensity. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2015;51:227–36. pmid:25462896
  33. 33. Kajantie E, Phillips DIW. The effects of sex and hormonal status on the physiological response to acute psychosocial stress. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2006;31(2):151–78. pmid:16139959
  34. 34. Bauman A, Ainsworth BE, Bull F, Craig CL, Hagstromer M, Sallis JF, et al. Progress and pitfalls in the use of the International Physical Activity Questionnaire (IPAQ) for adult physical activity surveillance. Journal of physical activity & health. 2009;6(1):S5.
  35. 35. Spielberger CD. State‐Trait Anxiety Inventory: Wiley Online Library; 2010.
  36. 36. Beck AT, Steer RA, Brown GK. Beck Depression Inventory. Manual, Swedish version. Sandviken: Psykologiförlaget; 2005.
  37. 37. AHA. Exercise testing and training of apparently healthy individuals: a handbook for physicians: American Heart Assoc.; 1972.
  38. 38. Lustyk MKB, Olson KC, Gerrish WG, Holder A, Widman L. Psychophysiological and neuroendocrine responses to laboratory stressors in women: Implications of menstrual cycle phase and stressor type. Biol Psychol. 2010;83(2):84–92. pmid:19922766
  39. 39. Collins A, Eneroth P, Landgren B-M. Psychoneuroendocrine stress responses and mood as related to the menstrual cycle. Psychosom Med. 1985;47(6):512–27. pmid:4070522
  40. 40. Hastrup JL, Light KC. Sex differences in cardiovascular stress responses: Modulation as a function of menstrual cycle phases. J Psychosom Res. 1984;28(6):475–83. pmid:6520803
  41. 41. Kirschbaum C, Kudielka BM, Gaab J, Schommer NC, Hellhammer DH. Impact of gender, menstrual cycle phase, and oral contraceptives on the activity of the hypothalamus-pituitary-adrenal axis. Psychosom Med. 1999;61(2):154–62. pmid:10204967
  42. 42. Manhem K, Jern C, Pilhall M, Shanks G, Jern S. Haemodynamic responses to psychosocial stress during the menstrual cycle. Clin Sci. 1991;81(1):17–22. pmid:1649720
  43. 43. Dettwyler K, Lohman TG, Roche AF, Martorell R. Anthropometric Standardization Reference Manual, Abridged Edition Am J Phys Anthropol. 1993;92(2):239–41.
  44. 44. Jayasinghe SU, Torres SJ, Fraser SF, Turner AI. Cortisol, blood pressure, and heart rate responses to food intake were independent of physical fitness levels in women. Appl Physiol Nutr Metab. 2015;40(11):1186–92. pmid:26499850
  45. 45. Jayasinghe S, Torres S, Nowson C, Tilbrook A, Turner A. Physiological responses to psychological stress: importance of adiposity in men aged 50–70 years. Endocrine connections. 2014;3(3):110–9. pmid:24867909
  46. 46. Pruessner JC, Kirschbaum C, Meinlschmid G, Hellhammer DH. Two formulas for computation of the area under the curve represent measures of total hormone concentration versus time-dependent change. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2003;28(7):916–31. pmid:12892658
  47. 47. Hellhammer J, Schubert M. The physiological response to Trier Social Stress Test relates to subjective measures of stress during but not before or after the test. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2012;37(1):119–24. pmid:21689890
  48. 48. Klaperski S, von Dawans B, Heinrichs M, Fuchs R. Does the level of physical exercise affect physiological and psychological responses to psychosocial stress in women? Psychol Sport Exerc. 2013;14(2):266–74.
  49. 49. Palatini P, Bratti P, Palomba D, Saladini F, Zanatta N, Maraglino G. Regular physical activity attenuates the blood pressure response to public speaking and delays the development of hypertension. J Hypertens. 2010;28(6):1186–93. pmid:20486274
  50. 50. Kirschbaum C, Strasburger C, Langkrär J. Attenuated cortisol response to psychological stress but not to CRH or ergometry in young habitual smokers. Pharmacol Biochem Behav. 1993;44(3):527–31. pmid:8451256
  51. 51. Carroll D, Phillips AC, Der G. Body mass index, abdominal adiposity, obesity, and cardiovascular reactions to psychological stress in a large community sample. Psychosom Med. 2008;70(6):653–60. pmid:18596249
  52. 52. Brinkmann K, Schüpbach L, Joye IA, Gendolla GH. Anhedonia and effort mobilization in dysphoria: Reduced cardiovascular response to reward and punishment. Int J Psychophysiol. 2009;74(3):250–8. pmid:19808061
  53. 53. Ginty AT, Phillips AC, Roseboom TJ, Carroll D, Derooij SR. Cardiovascular and cortisol reactions to acute psychological stress and cognitive ability in the Dutch Famine Birth Cohort Study. Psychophysiology. 2012;49(3):391–400. pmid:22091868
  54. 54. Silke B, McAuley D. Accuracy and precision of blood pressure determination with the Finapres: an overview using re-sampling statistics. J Hum Hypertens. 1998;12(6):403–9. pmid:9705043
  55. 55. Imholz BP, Wieling W, van Montfrans GA, Wesseling KH. Fifteen years experience with finger arterial pressure monitoring. Cardiovasc Res. 1998;38(3):605–16. pmid:9747429
  56. 56. Schutte AE, Huisman HW, van Rooyen JM, Oosthuizen W, Jerling JC. Sensitivity of the Finometer device in detecting acute and medium-term changes in cardiovascular function. Blood Press Monit. 2003;8(5):195–201. pmid:14624169
  57. 57. Shvartz E, Reibold R. Aerobic fitness norms for males and females aged 6 to 75 years: a review. Aviat Space Environ Med. 1990;61(1):3–11. pmid:2405832