Hostname: page-component-7c8c6479df-fqc5m Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-03-28T10:18:56.655Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Examining the Prospective Relationship between Family Affective Responsiveness and Theory of Mind in Chronic Paediatric Traumatic Brain Injury

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  11 October 2016

Nicholas P. Ryan*
Affiliation:
Australian Centre for Child Neuropsychological Studies, Murdoch Children's Research Institute, Melbourne, Victoria, Australia Melbourne School of Psychological Sciences, University of Melbourne, Melbourne, Victoria, Australia Department of Psychology, Royal Children's Hospital, Melbourne, Victoria, Australia
Kim Mihaljevic
Affiliation:
Australian Centre for Child Neuropsychological Studies, Murdoch Children's Research Institute, Melbourne, Victoria, Australia Melbourne School of Psychological Sciences, University of Melbourne, Melbourne, Victoria, Australia
Miriam H. Beauchamp
Affiliation:
Australian Centre for Child Neuropsychological Studies, Murdoch Children's Research Institute, Melbourne, Victoria, Australia Department of Psychology, University of Montreal, Montreal, Canada Resarch Centre, Ste-Justine Hospital, Montreal, Canada
Cathy Catroppa
Affiliation:
Australian Centre for Child Neuropsychological Studies, Murdoch Children's Research Institute, Melbourne, Victoria, Australia Department of Psychology, Royal Children's Hospital, Melbourne, Victoria, Australia
Louise Crossley
Affiliation:
Australian Centre for Child Neuropsychological Studies, Murdoch Children's Research Institute, Melbourne, Victoria, Australia
Stephen Hearps
Affiliation:
Australian Centre for Child Neuropsychological Studies, Murdoch Children's Research Institute, Melbourne, Victoria, Australia
Timothy Silk
Affiliation:
Australian Centre for Child Neuropsychological Studies, Murdoch Children's Research Institute, Melbourne, Victoria, Australia
Celia Godfrey
Affiliation:
Australian Centre for Child Neuropsychological Studies, Murdoch Children's Research Institute, Melbourne, Victoria, Australia
Keith Owen Yeates
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, Hotchkiss Brain Institute, and Alberta Children's Hospital Research Institute, The University of Calgary, Calgary, Alberta, Canada
Vicki A. Anderson
Affiliation:
Australian Centre for Child Neuropsychological Studies, Murdoch Children's Research Institute, Melbourne, Victoria, Australia Melbourne School of Psychological Sciences, University of Melbourne, Melbourne, Victoria, Australia Department of Psychology, Royal Children's Hospital, Melbourne, Victoria, Australia
*
Address correspondence: Nicholas Ryan, BA (Hons.), Child Neuropsychology, c/o Murdoch Children's Research Institute, Flemington Road, Parkville, 3052, Australia. Phone: +61 437 12 4427. E-mail: nicholas.ryan@mcri.edu.au
Get access

Abstract

Childhood and adolescence coincide with rapid structural and functional maturation of brain networks implicated in Theory of Mind (ToM); however, the impact of paediatric traumatic brain injury (TBI) on the development of these higher order skills is not well understood. ToM can be partitioned into conative ToM, defined as the ability to understand how indirect speech acts involving irony and empathy are used to influence the mental or affective state of the listener; and affective ToM, concerned with understanding that facial expressions are often used for social purposes to convey emotions that we want people to think we feel. In a sample of 84 children with mild-severe TBI and 40 typically developing controls, this study examined the effect of paediatric TBI on affective and conative ToM; and evaluated the respective contributions of injury-related factors (injury severity/lesion location) and non-injury-related environmental variables (socio-economic status (SES)/family functioning) to long-term ToM outcomes. Results showed that the poorest ToM outcomes were documented in association with mild-complicated and moderate TBI, rather than severe TBI. Lesion location and SES did not significantly contribute to conative or affective ToM. Post-injury family affective responsiveness was the strongest and most significant predictor of conative ToM. Results suggest that clinicians should exercise caution when prognosticating based on early clinical indicators, and that group and individual-level outcome prediction should incorporate assessment of a range of injury- and non-injury-related factors. Moreover, the affective quality of post-injury family interactions represents a potentially modifiable risk factor, and might be a useful target for family-centred interventions designed to optimise social cognitive outcomes after paediatric TBI.

Type
Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Australasian Society for the Study of Brain Impairment 2016 

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Adolphs, R. (2009). The social brain: Neural basis of social knowledge. Annual Review of Psychology, 60 (4), 693.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Anderson, V., Beauchamp, M.H., Yeates, K.O., Crossley, L., Hearps, S.J., & Catroppa, C. (2013). Social competence at 6 months following childhood traumatic brain injury. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 19 (05), 539550.Google Scholar
Anderson, V., Spencer-Smith, M., & Wood, A. (2011). Do children really recover better? Neurobehavioural plasticity after early brain insult. Brain, 134 (Pt 8), 21972221. doi: 10.1093/brain/awr103.Google Scholar
Beauchamp, M.H., Ditchfield, M., Babl, F.E., Kean, M., Catroppa, C., Yeates, K.O., & Anderson, V. (2011). Detecting traumatic brain lesions in children: CT versus MRI versus susceptibility weighted imaging (SWI). Journal of Neurotrauma, 28 (6), 915927.Google Scholar
Bigler, E.D. (2013). Traumatic brain injury, neuroimaging, and neurodegeneration. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 7 (1), 395.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bigler, E.D., Abildskov, T.J., Petrie, J., Farrer, T.J., Dennis, M., Simic, N., . . . Owen Yeates, K. (2013). Heterogeneity of brain lesions in pediatric traumatic brain injury. Neuropsychology, 27 (4), 438451. doi: 10.1037/a0032837.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Blakemore, S.J. (2008). The social brain in adolescence. Nature Review Neuroscience, 9 (4), 267277.Google Scholar
Blakemore, S.J. (2011). Social‐cognitive development during adolescence. In Child psychology and psychiatry: Frameworks for practice, (2nd ed., pp. 6266), Chichester, United Kingdom: John Wiley & Sons.Google Scholar
Blakemore, S.-J., den Ouden, H., Choudhury, S., & Frith, C. (2007). Adolescent development of the neural circuitry for thinking about intentions. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 2 (2), 130139.Google Scholar
Blakemore, S.-J., & Mills, K. L. (2014). Is adolescence a sensitive period for sociocultural processing? Annual Review of Psychology, 65 (1), 187207.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Burnett, S., & Blakemore, S. J. (2009). The development of adolescent social cognition. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1167 (1), 5156.Google Scholar
Burnett, S., Sebastian, C., Kadosh, K.C., & Blakemore, S.-J. (2011). The social brain in adolescence: Evidence from functional magnetic resonance imaging and behavioural studies. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 35 (8), 16541664.Google Scholar
Chapman, L.A., Wade, S.L., Walz, N.C., Taylor, H.G., Stancin, T., & Yeates, K.O. (2010a). Clinically significant behavior problems during the initial 18 months following early childhood traumatic brain injury. Rehabilitation Psychology, 55 (1), 4857.Google Scholar
Chapman, L.A., Wade, S.L., Walz, N.C., Taylor, H.G., Stancin, T., & Yeates, K.O. (2010b). Clinically significant behavior problems during the initial 18 months following early childhood traumatic brain injury. Rehabilitation Psychology, 55 (1), 48.Google Scholar
Choudhury, S., Blakemore, S.-J., & Charman, T. (2006). Social cognitive development during adolescence. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 1 (3), 165174.Google Scholar
Coffey, C., & Figiel, G. (1991). Neuropsychiatric significance of subcortical encephalomalacia. Psychopathology and the Brain, 3 (11), 243264.Google Scholar
Dennis, M., Agostino, A., Taylor, H.G., Bigler, E.D., Rubin, K., Vannatta, K., . . . Yeates, K.O. (2013a). Emotional expression and socially modulated emotive communication in children with traumatic brain injury. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 19 (01), 3443.Google Scholar
Dennis, M., Simic, N., Bigler, E.D., Abildskov, T., Agostino, A., Taylor, H.G., . . . Stancin, T. (2013b). Cognitive, affective, and conative theory of mind (ToM) in children with traumatic brain injury. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience, 5 (1), 2539.Google Scholar
Dumontheil, I., Apperly, I.A., & Blakemore, S.J. (2010). Online usage of theory of mind continues to develop in late adolescence. Developmental Science, 13 (2), 331338.Google Scholar
Epstein, N.B., Baldwin, L.M., & Bishop, D.S. (1983). The McMaster family assessment device*. Journal of Marital and Family Therapy, 9 (2), 171180.Google Scholar
Gerring, J.P., & Wade, S. (2012). The essential role of psychosocial risk and protective factors in pediatric traumatic brain injury research. Journal of Neurotrauma, 29 (4), 621628.Google Scholar
Gogtay, N., Giedd, J.N., Lusk, L., Hayashi, K.M., Greenstein, D., Vaituzis, A.C., . . . Toga, A.W. (2004). Dynamic mapping of human cortical development during childhood through early adulthood. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 101 (21), 81748179.Google Scholar
Harrison, P., & Oakland, T. (2003). Adaptive behavior assessment system (ABAS-II). San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation.Google Scholar
Hein, G., & Singer, T. (2007). I feel how you feel but not always: The empathic brain and its modulation. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 18, 153158.Google Scholar
Herbet, G., Lafargue, G., Bonnetblanc, F., Moritz-Gasser, S., & Duffau, H. (2013). Is the right frontal cortex really crucial in the mentalizing network? A longitudinal study in patients with a slow-growing lesion. Cortex, 49 (10), 27112727.Google Scholar
Johnson, M.H., Griffin, R., Csibra, G., Halit, H., Farroni, T., De Haan, M., . . . Richards, J. (2005). The emergence of the social brain network: Evidence from typical and atypical development. Development and Psychopathology, 17 (03), 599619.Google Scholar
Kurowski, B.G., Taylor, H.G., Yeates, K.O., Walz, N.C., Stancin, T., & Wade, S.L. (2011). Caregiver ratings of long-term executive dysfunction and attention problems after early childhood traumatic brain injury: Family functioning is important. Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation, 3 (9), 836845.Google Scholar
Lalonde, G., Bernier, A., Beaudoin, C., Gravel, J., & Beauchamp, M.H. (2016). Investigating social functioning after early mild TBI: The quality of parent–child interactions. Journal of Neuropsychology. [Epub ahead of print]. doi:10.1111/jnp.12104.Google ScholarPubMed
Levan, A., Baxter, L., Kirwan, C.B., Black, G., & Gale, S.D. (2015). Right frontal pole cortical thickness and social competence in children with chronic traumatic brain injury: Cognitive proficiency as a mediator. The Journal of Head Trauma Rehabilitation, 30 (2), E24–E31. doi: 10.1097/htr.0000000000000040.Google Scholar
Molenberghs, P., Johnson, H., Henry, J.D., & Mattingley, J.B. (2016). Understanding the minds of others: A neuroimaging meta-analysis. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 65 (1), 276291.Google Scholar
Robinson, L. (2007). The role of family context in the development of emotion regulation. Social Development, 16 (2), 361388.Google Scholar
Rosema, S., Crowe, L., & Anderson, V. (2012). Social function in children and adolescents after traumatic brain injury: A systematic review 1989-2011. Journal of Neurotrauma, 29 (7), 12771291.Google Scholar
Ryan, N.P., Catroppa, C., Beare, R., Coleman, L., Ditchfield, M., Crossley, L., . . . Anderson, V.A. (2015a). Predictors of longitudinal outcome and recovery of pragmatic language and its relation to externalizing behaviour after pediatric traumatic brain injury. Brain Language, 142 (1), 8695. doi: 10.1016/j.bandl.2015.01.007.Google Scholar
Ryan, N.P., Catroppa, C., Beare, R., Silk, T.J., Crossley, L., Beauchamp, M.H., . . . Anderson, V.A. (2016). Theory of mind mediates the prospective relationship between abnormal social brain network morphology and chronic behavior problems after pediatric traumatic brain injury (TBI). Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 11 (4), 683692.Google Scholar
Ryan, N.P., Catroppa, C., Cooper, J.M., Beare, R., Ditchfield, M., Coleman, L., . . . Anderson, V.A. (2015b). The emergence of age-dependent social cognitive deficits after generalized insult to the developing brain: A longitudinal prospective analysis using susceptibility-weighted imaging. Human Brain Mapping, 36 (5), 16771691. doi: 10.1002/hbm.22729.Google Scholar
Ryan, N.P., Catroppa, C., Cooper, J.M., Beare, R., Ditchfield, M., Coleman, L., . . . Anderson, V.A. (2015c). Relationships between acute imaging biomarkers and theory of mind impairment in post-acute pediatric traumatic brain injury: A prospective analysis using susceptibility weighted imaging (SWI). Neuropsychologia, 66 (1), 3238. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2014.10.040.Google Scholar
Schmidt, A.T., Hanten, G.R., Li, X., Orsten, K.D., & Levin, H.S. (2010). Emotion recognition following pediatric traumatic brain injury: longitudinal analysis of emotional prosody and facial emotion recognition. Neuropsychologia, 48 (10), 28692877.Google Scholar
Schmidt, A.T., Orsten, K.D., Hanten, G.R., Li, X., & Levin, H.S. (2010). Family environment influences emotion recognition following paediatric traumatic brain injury. Brain Injury, 24 (13–14), 15501560.Google Scholar
Schwartz, L., Taylor, H.G., Drotar, D., Yeates, K.O., Wade, S.L., & Stancin, T. (2003). Long-term behavior problems following pediatric traumatic brain injury: Prevalence, predictors, and correlates. Journal of Pediatric Psychology, 28 (4), 251263.Google Scholar
Schwartz, O.S., Dudgeon, P., Sheeber, L.B., Yap, M.B., Simmons, J.G., & Allen, N.B. (2012). Parental behaviors during family interactions predict changes in depression and anxiety symptoms during adolescence. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 40 (1), 5971.Google Scholar
Sebastian, C.L., Fontaine, N.M., Bird, G., Blakemore, S.-J., De Brito, S.A., McCrory, E.J., & Viding, E. (2011). Neural processing associated with cognitive and affective theory of mind in adolescents and adults. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 7 (1), 5363.Google Scholar
Steinberg, L., & Morris, A.S. (2001). Adolescent development. Journal of Cognitive Education and Psychology, 2 (1), 5587.Google Scholar
Sodian, B., Thoermer, C., & Metz, U. (2007). Now I see it but you don't: 14-month-olds can represent another person's visual perspective. Developmental Science, 10, 199204.Google Scholar
Surian, L., Caldi, S., & Sperber, D. (2007). Attribution of beliefs by 13- month-old infants. Psychological Science, 18, 580586.Google Scholar
Taylor, H.G., Yeates, K.O., Wade, S.L., Drotar, D., Stancin, T., & Minich, N. (2002). A prospective study of short-and long-term outcomes after traumatic brain injury in children: behavior and achievement. Neuropsychology, 16 (1), 15.Google Scholar
Teasdale, G., & Jennett, B. (1974). Assessment of coma and impaired consciousness. Lancet, 2, 8184.Google Scholar
Trewin, D., Trewin, D.J., Pink, B.N., & Zealand, S.N. (2006). ANZSCO: Australian and New Zealand standard classification of occupations. Australian Bureau of Statistics: Belconnen, Australian Capital Territory, Australia.Google Scholar
Wade, S.L., Cassedy, A., Walz, N.C., Taylor, H.G., Stancin, T., & Yeates, K.O. (2011). The relationship of parental warm responsiveness and negativity to emerging behavior problems following traumatic brain injury in young children. Developmental Psychology, 47 (1), 119133. doi: 10.1037/a0021028.Google Scholar
Walz, N.C., Yeates, K.O., Wade, S.L., & Mark, E. (2009). Social information processing skills in adolescents with traumatic brain injury: Relationship with social competence and behavior problems. Journal of Pediatric Rehabilitation Medicine, 2 (4), 285295.Google Scholar
Wellman, H.M., Cross, D., & Watson, J. (2001). Meta-analysis of theory of mind development: The truth about false belief. Child Development, 72, 655684.Google Scholar
Wang, A.T., Lee, S.S., Sigman, M., & Dapretto, M. (2006). Developmental changes in the neural basis of interpreting communicative intent. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 1 (2), 107121.Google Scholar
Yeates, K.O., Bigler, E.D., Dennis, M., Gerhardt, C.A., Rubin, K.H., Stancin, T., . . . Vannatta, K. (2007). Social outcomes in childhood brain disorder: A heuristic integration of social neuroscience and developmental psychology. Psychological Bulletin, 133 (3), 535556.Google Scholar
Yeates, K.O., Swift, E., Taylor, H.G., Wade, S.L., Drotar, D., Stancin, T., & Minich, N. (2004). Short- and long-term social outcomes following pediatric traumatic brain injury. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 10 (3), 412426. doi: 10.1017/s1355617704103093.Google Scholar
Yeates, K.O., Taylor, H.G., Walz, N.C., Stancin, T., & Wade, S.L. (2010). The family environment as a moderator of psychosocial outcomes following traumatic brain injury in young children. Neuropsychology, 24 (3), 345.Google Scholar
Supplementary material: File

Ryan supplementary material

Appendix 1

Download Ryan supplementary material(File)
File 98.8 KB