Rain or shine: Happiness and risk-taking

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.qref.2014.10.004Get rights and content

Highlights

  • Sunshine has a significant impact on happiness using longitudinal data from Germany and the Netherlands.

  • Instrumenting happiness with sunshine, happy people are found to be more risk-averse.

  • This instrumental variable approach gives more clue about causation.

  • Happy people are more concerned about the future than the present.

  • Findings have important implications for future research.

Abstract

In this paper, we focus on the effects of weather, such as sunshine, as an exogenous shifter of happiness using happiness data at the individual level, and estimate sunshine as a predictor of happiness. Then we relate the predicted happiness to risk-taking. By doing so, we estimate a relationship, stronger than a simple correlation, between happiness and risky behavior. Weather changes, and sunshine in particular, have substantial influences on personal happiness. However, unexpected weather changes appear to be more important than expected changes for happiness. We include several risk measures such as subjective risk-taking and financial assets in our analysis. Happier people appear to be more risk-averse in general and more specifically in financial decisions, and choose accordingly safer investments. This might be explained by the fact that happy people take more time for making decisions and have more self-control. In addition, predicted happiness affects expectations about longevity and inflation. Happy people expect to live longer and accordingly seem more concerned about the future than the present, and expect less inflation.

Introduction

The empirical research to date has mixed evidence on the relationship between the optimism of people in positive affective states and risk-taking. Therefore, the first contribution of this paper is to examine the causal effect of self-reported happiness (life satisfaction) on risky behavior using survey data. The stated objective is therefore to establish a relationship stronger than a simple correlation, which is always a difficult task, given the usual omitted variables and reverse causality problems which applied researchers face.

The literature focusing on the influence of weather on financial variables is providing mixed results.1 As a second contribution, we focus on the effects of weather, sunshine, as an exogenous shifter of happiness using happiness data at the person level, and estimate sunshine as a predictor of happiness. We then relate predicted happiness to risk-taking. Establishing this relationship could explain the correlation between weather and financial variables found in the finance literature, and could also help to determine the extent to which the findings from this research should be incorporated into policy analysis.

Further to this mixed evidence discussed above, the channels through which weather might affect financial variables are also unknown. Therefore, we examine the potential channels to explain our findings such us discount rates and self-control which is the third contribution of this paper. Using an instrumental variables approach, we establish a relationship going from happiness to risk-taking behavior. In order to establish this relationship, we use exogenous regional sunshine as an instrument for current personal happiness. Measures of sunshine are accessed through the European Climate Assessment Dataset (ECAD). Our personal survey data come from the Dutch Household Survey (DHS), which is a panel of 4500 persons covering the period 1993–2006, and the German Socio-Economic Panel (GSOEP), which is a panel of 21,000 persons surveyed from 1984 to 2006. The surveys provide self-reported measures of well-being, such as responses to questions about how happy and satisfied individual respondents are with their lives, as well as, very detailed information on wealth and different measures of risky behavior.

Our empirical findings suggest that weather changes, and sunshine in particular, have substantial influences on personal happiness. However, unexpected weather changes appear to be more important than expected changes of weather for happiness. The paper considers risk measures such as subjective risk-taking and financial assets. Happier people appear to be more risk-averse both in financial decisions and in general life decisions, and choose accordingly safer investments. Happy people are more likely to have life insurance, savings accounts, and operating assets, but are less likely to own stocks and bonds. Happy people prefer investing less in shares, because they find them too risky.

Happy people take more time to make decisions and have more self-control. In addition, happiness affects subjective expectations about longevity and inflation. Happy people expect to live longer and accordingly seem more concerned about the future than the present, and expect less inflation. Happier people are more forward looking than less happy people. Therefore, one could expect that people who expect to live longer presumably have stronger incentives to save, which is confirmed by our results for the savings accounts.

The remainder of the paper is organized as follows. Section 2 provides an overview of the related economic literature on correlates of well-being and the impact of well-being on risk-taking behavior. Section 3 summarizes the data, and Section 4 provides the details about the empirical framework and the identification strategy. Section 5 presents the empirical results and Section 6 concludes.

Section snippets

Related literature

Happiness has been studied extensively in psychology for a long time. However, it was not until 1974 that it was considered by economists as a research concept (Easterlin, 1974), since when there has been a proliferation of studies on the relationships between various personal characteristics and happiness.2 Happiness has been found to be positively related to good health (Dolan, Peasgood,

Data

The data come from three main sources: the European Climate Assessment Dataset, the Dutch Household Survey and the German Socio-Economic Panel. The ECAD consists of long-term daily resolution climatic time series for over 40 countries from meteorological stations throughout Europe and the Mediterranean. Most series cover at least the period from 1946 to the present. These series include temperature, precipitation, humidity, sunshine, cloudiness, sea level pressure, and snow depth. Three

Empirical framework

We would like to estimate the relationship between indicators of risky behavior (and other personal outcomes) and happiness controlling for personal characteristics, state/province and year fixed effects using OLS6 and clustering the robust standard errors by state/province (this is applied in the rest of the estimations).7

Does sunshine affect happiness?

Table 1 compares three measures of sunshine in a happiness equation where happiness is on scale 1–5. The coefficient for the first row is 0.04 and the t-statistic is 3.4, suggesting that a one hour increase in unexpected sunshine increases individual happiness by 0.04 units. The F-statistic is 17.3 which is much higher than 10.12 The F-statistic is much higher for the

Validity of the instrument

A desirable instrument should satisfy the following conditions. (i) Excludable: Hansen's J-statistics for excludability also confirms this finding, and (ii) sunshine affects risky behavior only through happiness: one may argue that weather can also potentially shift beliefs (optimism versus pessimism), which in turn affects economic behavior. On theoretical grounds, most of people's psychological characteristics are available in the surveys and are very persistent. Further, psychological

Conclusion

The paper verifies that sunshine has a significant impact on happiness using longitudinal data from Germany and the Netherlands. Instrumenting individual happiness with regional sunshine, happy people are found to be more risk-averse in financial decisions and they prefer safer investment tools. Happy people are more likely to have life insurance, savings accounts, and operating assets, but are less likely to own stocks or bonds. Happy people also have less desire to invest in shares because

References (36)

  • C. Floros

    Stock market returns and the temperature effect: New evidence from Europe

    Applied Financial Economics Letters

    (2008)
  • B.S. Frey et al.

    What can economists learn from happiness research?

    Journal of Economic Literature

    (2002)
  • P. Frijters et al.

    The effects of climate on welfare and well-being in Russia

    Climatic Change

    (1998)
  • K. Fromme et al.

    Outcome expectancies and risk-taking behavior

    Cognitive Therapy and Research

    (1997)
  • J.R. Gerlach

    Macroeconomic news and stock market calendar and weather anomalies

    Journal of Financial Research

    (2007)
  • W.N. Goetzmann et al.

    Rain or shine: Where is the weather effect?

    European Financial Management

    (2005)
  • D. Hirshleifer et al.

    Good day sunshine: Stock returns and the weather

    Journal of Finance

    (2003)
  • C.-L. Huang et al.

    Are happy investors likely to be overconfident?

    Emerging Markets Finance and Trade

    (2008)
  • Cited by (24)

    • How does happiness relate to economic behaviour? A review of the literature

      2017, Journal of Behavioral and Experimental Economics
      Citation Excerpt :

      Two other similar experiments (McLeish and Oxoby, 2007; Drichoutis and Nayga, 2013) yielded insignificant findings on the effect of positive mood on time discounting. However, the field study of Guven and Hoxha (2015) also found support for a positive effect of mood on patience; using weather as an instrument for mood, they found happier people reported being more patient and willing to prioritise the future over the present (this may partly have been due to the fact that happier people assigned themselves much longer life-expectancies). While positive moods appear to increase patience, it is not quite clear that negative moods reduce it.

    View all citing articles on Scopus

    STATA is used for regressions.

    View full text