Elsevier

Drug and Alcohol Dependence

Volume 185, 1 April 2018, Pages 330-338
Drug and Alcohol Dependence

Full length article
Maternal and partner prenatal alcohol use and infant cognitive development

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2017.12.038Get rights and content

Highlights

  • A large pregnancy cohort study assessed alcohol use by mothers and partners.

  • No observable effects of prenatal alcohol exposure on infant cognition.

  • Alcohol use by partners showed similar patterns of association.

  • Findings were robust using multiple methods to control for confounding.

  • Interaction between alcohol, social factors and infant development likely.

Abstract

Background

Teratogenicity of heavy prenatal alcohol exposure is established, but uncertainty remains regarding the impact of moderate alcohol exposure on cognitive deficits in infants. Separating in utero effects from environmental confounding is a challenge for observational studies; consideration of alcohol use by partners as well as mothers may help clarify this. This study examined associations between prenatal alcohol use by both mothers and their partners and infant cognitive developmental outcomes at 12-months.

Methods

Pregnant women (n = 1331) and their partners (n = 699) were recruited from antenatal clinics of three metropolitan public hospitals in Australia, and completed detailed interviews about alcohol consumptions throughout pregnancy. Infants were assessed with the Bayley Scales of Infant Development − Third edition (Bayley) at 12-months of age.

Results

Alcohol use during pregnancy was reported by 65.7% of mothers and 84.1% of partners. Using multiple methods to adjust for confounding factors, no evidence for impaired cognitive ability associated with alcohol use by mothers or their partners was observed. Children born to women who drank low-levels of alcohol had slightly higher Bayley cognitive scores than those born to abstaining women. There was some evidence for an interaction between sociodemographic factors and prenatal alcohol exposure on infant cognitive outcomes.

Conclusion

This finding corroborates existing evidence to suggest there are no detrimental effects to infant cognitive development at 12-months of age following low-level prenatal alcohol exposure. Future prospective studies involving families of a broad range of backgrounds would be informative to clarify interaction between alcohol exposure and environmental factors on developmental outcomes.

Introduction

Alcohol has been established as a teratogen and may produce central nervous system deficits at high levels of exposure, yet uncertainty remains regarding the association between low-level prenatal alcohol exposure and impaired cognitive development. Most large-scale studies fail to detect any observable effect (Alati et al., 2008; Forrest et al., 1991; Kelly et al., 2009; Kesmodel et al., 2012; O'Callaghan et al., 2007; Williams Brown et al., 2010). A recent meta-analysis found evidence for lower IQ scores among children exposed to binge-level alcohol exposure, yet this effect was no longer statistically significant when only studies considered ‘high-quality’ were included (Flak et al., 2014). The same meta-analysis found a small but significant counterintuitive effect in with low-level exposure was associated with higher IQ scores in childhood (Flak et al., 2014). No association was found between binge or moderate exposure and impairment in other cognitive domains including attention, verbal ability, mathematical ability at school, language development, or visuo-spatial ability (Flak et al., 2014 supplement). Some studies, however, have shown the apparent ‘positive’ effect of low-level alcohol exposure on IQ is mitigated with appropriate adjustment for confounders such as maternal socioeconomic status and education (Brown et al., 2010; Jacobson et al., 1993; Kelly et al., 2009).

A major challenge facing research in this area is that systematic differences exist between women who drink alcohol during pregnancy and women who are non-drinkers on factors that are independently associated with child developmental outcomes. Furthermore, comparison of two systematic reviews reveals divergent sets of characteristics between those whose children have a diagnosis of FASD compared with those who tend to drink alcohol in pregnancy − an “epidemiological puzzle” (Meurk et al., 2014). A systematic review found that apart from pre-pregnancy alcohol consumption, older age, higher educational attainment, higher parity, being employed, and higher income were consistent predictors of increased likelihood to drink during pregnancy (Skagerström et al., 2013). Many of these factors describe a population of overall advantaged women whose offspring are likely to be raised in enriched environments. In contrast, a systematic review found that lower educational attainment, being single during pregnancy, unemployment, residing in a remote area, and lower income were all associated with having a child with FASD (Esper and Furtado, 2014). This is in line with the common finding that Fetal Alcohol Spectrum Disorders (FASD) are concentrated in disadvantaged, low SES populations (Abel, 1995; Abel and Hannigan, 1995; Burns et al., 2013; Chudley, 2008; Kvigne et al., 2003; Lange et al., 2013; May et al., 2000; May et al., 2004; May et al., 2009; Meurk et al., 2014). Older maternal age, substance use, smoking, and experience of trauma or violence were common risk factors for both FASD (Esper and Furtado, 2014) and prenatal alcohol exposure (Skagerström et al., 2013). At an epidemiological level, these divergent sets of demographic predictors suggest some interaction between prenatal alcohol exposure and environmental factors in producing deficits. For this reason, it is critical that studies attempting to assess impacts of prenatal alcohol exposure collect comprehensive data on psychosocial and demographic factors and include these in models.

Random allocation to an alcohol exposure condition in pregnancy is impossible; therefore, confounding in observational studies is dealt with statistically by inclusion of covariates in regression models. Propensity Score Matching is an alternative technique, used when randomisation to conditions is not possible, which ensures baseline characteristics are consistent between groups and any differences between them attributable to ‘treatment’ or exposure effects (Austin, 2011; Rosenbaum and Rubin, 1985). The Millennium Cohort Study in the UK observed no difference in cognitive performance between children with low-level prenatal alcohol exposure and those not exposed, and this was regardless of whether ordinary least-squares regression or propensity score matching was used to account for bias (Kelly et al., 2013).

Another method by which the impact of shared postnatal family environment can be disentangled from that of the intrauterine environment is to compare magnitude of associations between maternal alcohol use and offspring outcomes with that of alcohol use by partners (Alati et al., 2008; Smith, 2008). Some studies using animal models suggest paternal effects of pre-conception alcohol use on offspring, including lower birth weight (Abel, 2004; Meek et al., 2007), and some suggest cognitive deficits in early life (Abel, 2004). In human research, one prospective cohort study found that while moderate alcohol use by mothers predicted poorer language ability in children, this effect was no greater in magnitude than the association with alcohol use by fathers (Alati et al., 2008). However, a subsequent follow-up of the same sample found a specific effect of frequent, heavy alcohol consumption by mothers and associated lower academic abilities in offspring (Alati et al., 2013). Few other prospective longitudinal studies have accounted for the effect of partner alcohol consumption when studying effects of prenatal alcohol exposure on child development.

The aims of this paper were to (1) examine the relationship between prenatal alcohol exposure across four time points during pregnancy on infant cognitive development at 12 months of age and (2) assess the relationship between alcohol consumption by partners and infant cognitive development.

Section snippets

Participants

Data were drawn from The Triple B Study (Bumps, Babies and Beyond), a prospective pregnancy cohort study. This study was approved by the Sydney Local Health District Research Ethics and Governance Office, and this was ratified by Ethics Committees at participating hospital and university sites. Pregnant women were recruited between 2008 and 2013 from waiting rooms of antenatal clinics at metropolitan public hospitals in New South Wales and Western Australia. All women in waiting rooms on

Prevalence of alcohol consumption by mothers and partners

Alcohol use at any time during pregnancy was reported by the majority (65.7%) of women (Table 1). In the first 6 weeks of Trimester 1, consumption at binge and heavy levels was the most common pattern, with 27.3% and 23.1% of drinkers (16.5% and 14.0% of total sample) falling into these categories, respectively. Alcohol consumption was markedly less common and occurred at lower levels in the latter half of Trimester 1, with 72.3% of women abstaining from alcohol completely and the majority of

Discussion

Infants born to women who drank at low levels during Trimester 2 had higher cognitive ability at 12-months of age than those born to abstainers; this finding was robust and significant, even in samples matched on all baseline characteristics distinguishing drinkers from abstainers via propensity score matching. This is not the first study to report this finding; the meta-analysis by Flak et al. (2014) made the same finding when pooling results of high-quality studies. In a systematic review by

Funding

The research was funded by an Australian National Health and Medical Research Council (NHMRC) Project Grant #GNT630517 to RPM, DH, SA, JN, EE, LB, SJ, CO, and AB, and was financially supported by the National Drug and Alcohol Research Centre (NDARC), University of New South Wales (UNSW). The study has also been supported by Australian Rotary Health (ARH; 2012–2013) the Foundation for Alcohol Research and Education (FARE; 2010–2011), and the Financial Markets Foundation for Children (Australia)

Contributors

CM conceived the study, collected data, conducted analyses and wrote the manuscript under supervision of DH, RM and LB. DH, RM, LB, EE, CO, JN, SA, SJ conceived the parent study, obtained funding, oversaw research and provided critical review of the manuscript and approved the final manuscript. GY and JW assisted with data analyses and drafts of the manuscript, provided critical review and approved the final manuscript.

LR collected data, provided critical review and approved the final

Conflict of interest

No conflict declared.

Acknowledgements

We gratefully acknowledge the investigators of the Triple B Study not listed as authors and the NDARC research staff who collected the data. We also wish to acknowledge the hospitals and antenatal clinics for their assistance with recruitment, and the study participants and their families. We also wish to acknowledge the Longitudinal Cohorts Research Consortium (LCRC); NHMRC Project Grants: AAP1009381, AAP1064893; formally the Cannabis Cohorts Research Consortium (CCRC).

References (51)

  • C.A. Albers et al.

    Test review: Bayley, N. 2006. Bayley scales of infant and toddler development–third edition. San Antonio, TX: Harcourt assessment

    J. Psychoeduc. Assess.

    (2007)
  • P.D. Allison
    (2001)
  • P.C. Austin

    A critical appraisal of propensity-score matching in the medical literature between 1996 and 2003

    Stat. Med.

    (2008)
  • P.C. Austin

    An introduction to propensity score methods for reducing the effects of confounding in observational studies

    Multivar. Behav. Res.

    (2011)
  • Australian Bureau of Statistics

    Census of Population and Housing: Socio-economic Indexes for Areas (SEIFA)

    (2011)
  • N. Bayley
    (2006)
  • C.W. Brown et al.

    Maternal alcohol consumption during pregnancy and infant social, mental, and motor development

    J. Early Interv.

    (2010)
  • L. Burns et al.

    Counting fetal alcohol spectrum disorder in Australia: the evidence and the challenges

    Drug Alcohol Rev.

    (2013)
  • A.E. Chudley

    Fetal alcohol spectrum disorder: counting the invisible–mission impossible?

    Arch. Dis. Child.

    (2008)
  • J.L. Cox et al.

    Detection of postnatal depression: development of the 10-item edinburgh postnatal depression scale

    Br. J. Psychiatry

    (1987)
  • L.H. Esper et al.

    Identifying maternal risk factors associated with fetal alcohol spectrum disorders: a systematic review

    Eur. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry

    (2014)
  • A.L. Flak et al.

    The association of mild, moderate, and binge prenatal alcohol exposure and child neuropsychological outcomes: a meta-analysis

    Alcohol. Clin. Exp. Res.

    (2014)
  • F. Forrest et al.

    Reported social alcohol consumption during pregnancy and infants’ development at 18 months

    BMJ

    (1991)
  • J.W. Graham

    Missing data analysis: making it work in the real world

    Ann. Rev. Psychol.

    (2009)
  • J. Henderson et al.

    Systematic review of effects of low-moderate prenatal alcohol exposure on pregnancy outcome

    BJOG

    (2007)
  • Cited by (20)

    • Early life multiple exposures and child cognitive function: A multi-centric birth cohort study in six European countries

      2021, Environmental Pollution
      Citation Excerpt :

      This finding is not consistent with previous studies (Scott-Goodwin et al., 2016). However, a recent cohort study reported that moderate alcohol intake during pregnancy was not associated with child cognitive development at 12 months (McCormack et al., 2018). Data in the European cohorts of this study, showed that moderate alcohol consumption was more prevalent among highly educated mothers and the reported association was basically driven by this group.

    • An investigation of the link between prenatal alcohol exposure and sleep problems across childhood

      2021, Drug and Alcohol Dependence
      Citation Excerpt :

      The abolishment of two of the three effects in the current study suggests that the positive effect of occasional drinking stems from residual variance associated with being from a higher SES/lower adversity background. This residual variance may not have been fully captured in previous studies (McCormack et al., 2018) and perhaps was captured better in this study owing to the inclusion of parenting and family stress factors. Several covariates contributed significantly to the occurrence of sleep problems across the 2- to 9-year period.

    • Prenatal Risk Factors and Neurodevelopment

      2020, Encyclopedia of Infant and Early Childhood Development
    • Prenatal alcohol exposure and facial morphology in a UK cohort

      2019, Drug and Alcohol Dependence
      Citation Excerpt :

      The implication of these results is that there is no strong evidence for an effect of alcohol exposure on the FAS-related facial phenotypes tested in our study sample. Our findings are consistent with the well-characterized difficulties of identifying the effects of prenatal alcohol exposure (Suttie et al., 2013) as well as with the findings of previous studies that found no clear evidence of an association between prenatal alcohol exposure and adverse effects (Flak et al., 2014; Kelly et al., 2013; Mamluk et al., 2017; McCormack et al., 2018). Contrastingly, a previous investigation by Muggli et al. found evidence of sub-clinical facial differences at 12 months between offspring of non-drinking mothers and offspring of light to moderate drinking mothers (Muggli et al., 2017).

    View all citing articles on Scopus
    View full text