Elsevier

Appetite

Volume 101, 1 June 2016, Pages 199-204
Appetite

The influence of a high-fat meal on fat taste thresholds

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.appet.2016.03.011Get rights and content

Abstract

A high-fat diet for four weeks has been shown to attenuate fat taste sensitivity in healthy weight individuals. However, there is minimal evidence as to whether a single high-fat meal immediately prior to fat taste threshold testing has an effect on thresholds. Therefore, the aim of the study was to determine the effect of a high-fat meal immediately prior to detection threshold testing for oleic acid (C18:1). Thirty-two participants (15 males, 17 females, aged 39.1 ± 3.1 years, Body Mass Index 23.1 ± 0.7 kg/m2) attended three laboratory sessions. In each session, participants were randomly assigned to one of three different types of breakfast: a high-fat (60% energy from fat), or low-fat (20% energy from fat) or macronutrient balanced (33% energy from fat) frittata. Fat taste thresholds were evaluated using ascending forced choice triangle tests on two occasions each day; once one-hour post breakfast and then one-hour post the completion of the first threshold test. There was no effect of breakfast type on fat taste detection thresholds for the first testing session of each day (P = 0.288), or the second testing session of each day (P = 0.754). There was also no effect of breakfast within each day (day 1: P = 0.198, day 2: P = 0.199, day 3: P = 0.125). There was no effect of macronutrient composition on the ability of participants to rank the level of fat in food (P = 0.345), or preference for the level of fat in food (P = 0.187–0.868). This study provides preliminary evidence that the composition of the meal consumed by a participant immediately prior to testing does not affect fat taste thresholds.

Introduction

Emerging evidence from humans and animals suggests a taste modality responsive to fat (Keast and Constanzo, 2015, Running et al., 2013). Mounting evidence is suggestive of a link between dietary fat consumption and sensitivity to fatty acids. Research has focussed on the potential association between excess dietary fat consumption and sensitivity to fatty acids throughout the alimentary canal (Little and Feinle-Bisset, 2011, Stewart et al., 2011). This work also suggested that oral fatty acid sensitivity is linked to body mass index (BMI) with hypersensitive individuals having lower BMIs than hyposensitive individuals. Nevertheless, this association remains contentious with some other studies finding no association between BMI and oral fatty acid sensitivity (Kamphuis et al., 2003, Mattes, 2009, Mattes, 2011, Stewart and Keast, 2012).

Adaptation of the taste system has been shown to occur with some nutrients including sodium chloride (NaCl) where increasing the amount of dietary sodium increases the taste detection threshold of NaCl (Mattes, 1997). Similarly, fat taste thresholds may be influenced by long-term fat consumption with modulation occurring after consumption of a modified diet over a 4-week period (Newman et al., 2016, Stewart and Keast, 2012); however, there is little evidence surrounding the fat content of a meal immediately prior to testing being a contributing factor to intra-individual variation in fat taste detection thresholds (Tucker, Nuessle, Garneau, Smutzer, & Mattes, 2015).

There have been numerous studies investigating the influence of meal consumption on primary taste thresholds, but results are variable with some studies showing an effect (Moore et al., 1965, Suchecka et al., 2011, Zverev, 2004), while others showing no effect (Pasquet, Monneuse, Simmen, Marez, & Hladik, 2006). Pasquet et al. (Pasquet et al., 2006) investigated the effect of a test meal on detection and recognition thresholds for the primary tastes and found no differences in taste thresholds for sucrose, fructose, sodium chloride (NaCl), 6-n-propylthiouracil (PROP) and quinine pre- and post-meal. However, Moore et al. (Moore et al., 1965) reported that there was a trend for a reduced taste of sucrose after a meal (the macronutrient composition of the meal was not discussed). Similarly, another study reported that consumption of a high-carbohydrate meal influences taste perception of sweet taste in men, but not women (Suchecka et al., 2011). The same study also reported that salty taste was not influenced by the high-carbohydrate meal (Suchecka et al., 2011), suggesting that it may be a macronutrient specific effect. The composition of the meals provided to the participants in these studies may impact on the differing results found for example, a meal high in sodium may directly increase or decrease sodium thresholds, however a meal high in sucrose may have no effect on sodium thresholds. In addition, Tucker et al. (Tucker et al., 2015) investigated this concept with regards to fat taste and reported a negative correlation between the total amount of fat and monounsaturated fat consumed in a meal prior to testing and taste intensity ratings for linoleic acid. Thus, these findings suggest there is a possible association between dietary fat and fat taste thresholds which warrants further investigation. Therefore, this study aimed to determine the effect of a high-fat (60% fat: 20% carbohydrate: 20% protein), low-fat (20% fat: 40% carbohydrate: 40% protein) or macronutrient balanced (33.3% fat: 33.3% carbohydrate: 33.3% protein) meal on fat taste thresholds.

Section snippets

Participants

Power analysis was conducted prior to the study to determine an appropriate sample size to achieve adequate statistical power. Data were used from a prior study which evaluated detection thresholds for fatty acids (Stewart et al., 2010). Using an α level of 0.05 and a 10% β (90% power), it was predicted that 31 participants would be required for this study to detect a C18:1 detection threshold difference of 0.65 mM. Participants were recruited from Deakin University, Burwood, Victoria,

Participants

Participants (n = 32, 15 males: age 49.3 ± 4.8 years [range 22–73 years], BMI 24.7 ± 0.8 kg/m2 [range 20.8–30.5 kg/m2], 17 females: age 31.5 ± 2.8 years [range 22–60 years], BMI 21.86 ± 0.9 kg/m2 [range 18.3–33.8 kg/m2]) were recruited. Out of the total 32 participants, 7 were classified as overweight/obese (n = 7, 5 male, 2 female, BMI 29.1 ± 0.8 kg/m2 [range 25.5–33.7 kg/m2]).

Concentrations of C18:1 fatty acid in test meals

The frittatas contained varying amounts of fat (high-fat frittata contained a total of 9.7 g/100 g of fat and 2.4 g

Discussion

This study demonstrated that fat taste thresholds were stable and not influenced by consumption of a high-fat, low-fat or macronutrient balanced breakfast both one and two hours post consumption. This study also added to the knowledge base of the reliability of the fat taste threshold measurement where there was a high test-retest reliability within participants. Participants' ability to detect the fat content of custard, or preference for regular- and low-fat foods did not differ between

Conflicts of interest

None.

Author contributions

The author contributions were as follows: L.P.N was involved in the study conception and design, collection, analysis and interpretation of data and drafting of the manuscript. S.J.T provided expert input and guidance. D.P.B provided expert input and was involved in drafting of the manuscript. R.S.J.K was involved in the study conception and design, collection, analysis and interpretation of data and drafting of the manuscript and revising it critically for important intellectual content. All

Acknowledgements

This work was funded by the Strategic Research Cluster for Physical Activity and Nutrition Research, School of Exercise and Nutrition Sciences, Deakin University, Victoria, Australia. Lisa Newman was funded by an Australian Postgraduate Award provided by The Department of Education Employment and Workplace Relations.

References (22)

  • M.M.J.W. Kamphuis et al.

    The effect of addition of linoleic acid on food intake regulation in linoleic acid tasters and linoleic acid non-tasters

    British Journal of Nutrition

    (2003)
  • Cited by (8)

    • Individual differences in oral tactile sensitivity and gustatory fatty acid sensitivity and their relationship with fungiform papillae density, mouth behaviour and texture perception of a food model varying in fat

      2021, Food Quality and Preference
      Citation Excerpt :

      Despite these limitations, the conclusions drawn are somewhat in-line, indirectly, with previous studies. Several studies examined the relationship between BMI and fatty acid sensitivity (Chevrot et al., 2014; Karmous et al., 2018; Keast, Azzopardi, Newman, & Haryono, 2014; Kindleysides et al., 2017; Newman, Torres, Bolhuis, & Keast, 2016; Stewart et al., 2010; Tucker et al., 2014, 2015; Zhou et al., 2016). In these studies, Stewart et al. (2010) found that subjects hypersensitive to oleic acid had a lower BMI and proposed that oral fatty acid hypersensitivity was associated with lower energy and fat intakes and lower BMI.

    • Detection thresholds for four different fatty stimuli are associated with increased dietary intake of processed high-caloric food

      2018, Appetite
      Citation Excerpt :

      In the current study, parameters such as BMI, waist circumference and WHR were not associated with detection thresholds for oleic acid, paraffin oil, canola oil and canola oil spiked with oleic acid. This observation is in line with previous studies (Mattes, 2011; Tucker et al., 2013; Chevrot et al., 2014; Martinez-Cordero, Malacara-Hernandez & Martinez-Cordero, 2015; Tucker, Nuessle Garneau & Mattes, 2015; Newman, Torres, Bolhuis & Keast, 2016b) and a recent meta-analysis (Tucker et al., 2017). In contrast, other studies found a positive association between BMI and fat detection thresholds (Stewart et al., 2010; Stewart et al., 2011a; Stewart et al., 2011b; Stewart & Keast, 2012; Tucker et al., 2014; Daoudi et al., 2015; Sayed et al., 2015).

    • Reduced taste responsiveness and increased food neophobia characterize obese adults

      2018, Food Quality and Preference
      Citation Excerpt :

      Although in the present study food preferences and dietary intake have not been considered, we can speculate that subjects with higher BMI could prefer high energy dense foods, rich in fat and sugar, to compensate their altered/reduced sensitivity (Bertoli et al., 2014; Proserpio et al., 2016), influencing their body weight. Moreover, recent evidence has shown that, in a small cohort of obese subjects, reduction in dietary fat content for 6 weeks improved taste sensitivity to fat (Newman, Torres, Bolhuis, & Keast, 2016), highlighting the likely association between taste perception, food preference and consumption. Similarly, Wise, Nattress, Flammer, and Beauchamp (2016) showed that the reduction of simple sugars in a group of obese subjects increased the perceived sweet taste intensity without decreasing the pleasantness.

    • Mastication of hard gumi decreases the gustatory threshold for sodium chloride

      2020, Journal of Nutritional Science and Vitaminology
    View all citing articles on Scopus
    View full text