Skip to main content
Log in

Superior Visual Search and Crowding Abilities Are Not Characteristic of All Individuals on the Autism Spectrum

  • Original Paper
  • Published:
Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Individuals with Autism Spectrum Disorder (ASD) often excel on visual search and crowding tasks; however, inconsistent findings suggest that this ‘islet of ability’ may not be characteristic of the entire spectrum. We examined whether performance on these tasks changed as a function of motor proficiency in children with varying levels of ASD symptomology. Children with high ASD symptomology outperformed all others on complex visual search tasks, but only if their motor skills were rated at, or above, age expectations. For the visual crowding task, children with high ASD symptomology and superior motor skills exhibited enhanced target discrimination, whereas those with high ASD symptomology but poor motor skills experienced deficits. These findings may resolve some of the discrepancies in the literature.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Institutional subscriptions

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3
Fig. 4
Fig. 5
Fig. 6

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Akshoomoff, N. A., & Courchesne, E. (1992). A new role for the cerebellum in cognitive operations. Behavioural Neuroscience, 106(5), 731–738.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Akshoomoff, N. A., & Courchesne, E. (1994). ERP evidence for a shifting attention deficit in patients with damage to the cerebellum. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 6(4), 388–399.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Almeida, R. A., Dickinson, J. E., Maybery, M. T., Badcock, J. C., & Badcock, D. R. (2010). Visual search performance in the autism spectrum II: The radial frequency search task with additional segmentation cues. Neuropsychologia, 48(14), 4117–4124.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Almeida, R. A., Dickinson, J. E., Maybery, M. T., Badcock, J. C., & Badcock, D. R. (2013). Visual search targeting either local or global perceptual processes differs as a function of autistic-like traits in the typically developing population. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 43, 1272–1286.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • APA (2013). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (5th ed.). Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Publishing.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ashwin, C., Wheelwright, S., & Baron-Cohen, S. (2006). Finding a face in the crowd: Testing the anger superiority effect in asperger syndrome. Brain and Cognition, 61(1), 78–95.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Asperger, H. (1938). Das psychisch abnorme Kind. Wiener Klinische Wochenschrift, 49, 1314–1317.

    Google Scholar 

  • Asperger, H. (1944). Die ‘autistischen Psychopathen’ im Kindesalter. Archiv fur Psychiatrie und Nervenkrankheiten, 117, 76–136.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Baldassi, S., Pei, F., Megna, N., Recupero, G., Viespoli, M., Igliozzi, R., et al. (2009). Search superiority in autism within but not outside the crowding regime. Vision Research, 49(16), 2151–2156.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Baumann, O., Borra, R. J., Bower, J. M., Cullen, K. E., Habas, C., Ivry, R. B., et al. (2015). Consensus paper: The role of the cerebellum in perceptual processes. Cerebellum, 14, 197–220.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bayliss, A. P., & Kritikos, A. (2011). Brief report: Perceptual load and the autism spectrum in typically developed individuals. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 41, 1573–1578.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Blaser, E., Eglington, L., Carter, A. S., & Kaldy, Z. (2014). Pupillometry reveals a mechanism for the autism spectrum disorder (ASD) advantage in visual tasks. Nature Scientific Reports, 4, 1–5.

    Google Scholar 

  • Blaser, E., Eglington, L., & Kaldy, Z. (2012) Toddlers with ASD are better at visual search without trying harder: A pupillometric study. In Annual Meeting of the Vision Sciences Society, Naples, FL, 2012.

  • Bouma, H. (1970). Interaction effects in parafoveal letter recognition. Nature, 226, 177–178.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Brock, J., Xu, J. Y., & Brooks, K. R. (2011). Individual differences in visual search: Relationship to autistic traits, discrimination thresholds, and speed of processing. Perception, 40(6), 739–742.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Buckner, R. L. (2013). The cerebellum and cognitive function: 25 years of insight from anatomy and neuroimaging. Neuron, 80(3), 807–815.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Caron, M. J., Mottron, L., Berthiaume, C., & Dawson, M. (2006). Cognitive mechanisms, specificity and neural underpinnings of visuospatial peaks in autism. Brain, 129(Pt 7), 1789–1802.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chakravarthi, R., & Cavanagh, P. (2007). Temporal properties of the polarity advantage effect in crowding. Journal of Vision, 7(2), 1–13.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chakravarthi, R., & Cavanagh, P. (2009). Bilateral field advantage in visual crowding. Vision Research, 49(13), 1638–1646.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Conners, K. C. (2008). Conners (3rd ed.). Toronto: Multi-Health Systems.

    Google Scholar 

  • Constable, P. (2010). Crowding and visual search in high functioning adults with autism spectrum disorder. Clinical Optometry, 2, 93–103.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Constantino, J. N., & Gruber, C. P. (2012). Social responsiveness scale-second edition (SRS-2). Torrance, CA: Western Psychological Services.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dakin, S., & Frith, U. (2005). Vagaries of visual perception in autism. Neuron, 48(3), 497–507.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Donovan, A. P., & Basson, M. A. (2017). The neuroanatomy of autism: A developmental perspective. Journal of Anatomy, 230(1), 4–15.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fielding, J., Corben, L., Cremer, P., Millist, L., White, O., & Delatycki, M. (2010). Disruption to higher order processes in Friedreich ataxia. Neuropsychologia, 48(1), 235–242.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Freyberg, J., Robertson, C. E., & Baron-Cohen, S. (2016). Typical magnitude and spatial extent of crowding in autism. Journal of Vision, 16(5), 1–10.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Green, D., Chairman, T., Pickles, A., Chandler, S., Loucas, T., Simonoff, E., et al. (2009). Impairment in movement skills of children with autistic spectrum disorders. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology, 51(4), 311–316.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Grubb, M. A., Behrmann, M., Egan, E., Minshew, N. J., Heeger, D. J., & Carrasco, M. (2013). Exogenous spatial attention: Evidence for intact functioning in adults with autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Vision, 13(14), 1–13.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hampson, D. R., & Blatt, G. J. (2015). Autism spectrum disorders and neuropathology of the cerebellum. Frontiers in Neuroscience, 9, 1–16.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Harrison, W. J., Mattingley, J. B., & Remington, R. W. (2013). Eye movement targets are released from visual crowding. The Journal of Neuroscience, 33(7), 2927–2933.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hayes, A. F. (2013). Introduction to mediation, moderation, and conditional process analysis: A regression-based approach. New York: The Guildford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Henderson, S. E., Sugden, D. A., & Barnett, A. L. (2007). Movement assessment battery for children-2: Movement ABC-2: Examiner’s manual. São Paulo: Pearson.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hocking, D. R., Corben, L. A., Fielding, J., Cremer, P. D., Millist, L., White, O. B., et al. (2014). Saccade reprogramming in Friedreich ataxia reveals impairments in the cognitive control of saccadic eye movement. Brain and Cognition, 87, 161–167.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hocking, D. R., Fielding, J., Corben, L. A., Cremer, P. D., Millist, L., White, O. B., et al. (2010). Ocular motor fixation deficits in Friedreich ataxia. Cerebellum, 9(3), 411–418.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hokkanen, L. S. K., Kauranen, V., Roine, R. O., Salonen, O., & Kotila, M. (2006). Subtle cognitive deficits after cerebellar infarcts. European Journal of Neurology, 13, 161–170.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Iarocci, G., & Armstrong, K. (2014). Age-related changes in conjunctive visual search in children with and without ASD. Autism Research, 7(2), 229–236.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Iarocci, G., Burack, J. A., Shore, D. I., Mottron, L., & Enns, J. T. (2006). Global-local visual processing in high functioning children with autism: Structural vs. implicit task biases. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 36(1), 117–129.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Intriligator, J., & Cavanagh, P. (2001). The spatial resolution of visual attention. Cognitive Psychology, 43(3), 171–216.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jarrold, C., Gilchrist, I. D., & Bender, A. (2005). Embedded figures detection in autism and typical development: Preliminary evidence of a double dissociation in relationships with visual search. Developmental Science, 8(4), 344–351.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Johnson, B. P., Rinehart, N. J., Papadopoulos, N., Tonge, B., Millist, L., White, O., et al. (2012). A closer look at visually guided saccades in autism and Asperger’s disorder. Frontiers in Integrative Neuroscience, 6, 99.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Johnson, B. P., Rinehart, N. J., White, O., Millist, L., & Fielding, J. (2013). Saccade adaptation in autism and Asperger’s disorder. Neuroscience, 243, 76–87.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Joseph, R. M., Keehn, B., Connolly, C., Wolfe, J. M., & Horowitz, T. S. (2009). Why is visual search superior in autism spectrum disorder? Developmental Science, 12(6), 1083–1096.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kaldy, Z., Giserman, I., Carter, A. S., & Blaser, E. (2016). The mechanisms underlying the ASD advantage in visual search. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-013-1957-x.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Kaldy, Z., Kraper, C., Carter, A. S., & Blaser, E. (2011). Toddlers with autism spectrum disorder are more successful at visual search than typically developing toddlers. Developmental Science, 14(5), 980–988.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kanner, L. (1943). Autistic disturbances of affective contact. Nervous Child, 2, 217–250.

    Google Scholar 

  • Keehn, B., Muller, R. A., & Townsend, J. (2013). Atypical attentional networks and the emergence of autism. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 37(2), 164–183.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kéïta, L., Mottron, L., & Bertone, A. (2010). Far visual acuity is unremarkable in autism: Do we need to focus on crowding? Autism Research, 3(6), 333–341.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Levy, S. E., Giarelli, E., Lee, L.-C., Schieve, L. A., Kirby, R. S., Cunniff, C., et al. (2010). Autism spectrum disorder and co-occurring developmental, psychiatric, and medical conditions among children in multiple populations of the United States. Journal of Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics, 31, 267–275.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Liss, M., Saulnier, C., Fein, D., & Kinsbourne, M. (2006). Sensory and attention abnormalities in autistic spectrum disorders. Autism, 10(2), 155–172.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Milne, E., Dunn, S. A., Freeth, M., & Rosas-Martinez, L. (2013). Visual search performance is predicted by the degree to which selective attention to features modulates the ERP between 350 and 600 ms. Neuropsychologia, 51(6), 1109–1118.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nieto, A., Correia, R., de Nóbreega, E., Montón, F., Hess, S., & Barroso, J. (2012). Cognition in friedreich ataxia. Cerebellum, 11, 834–844.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • O’Halloran, C. J., Kinsella, G. J., & Storey, E. (2012). The cerebellum and neuropsychological functioning: A critical review. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 34(1), 35–56.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • O’Riordan, M. (2000). Superior modulation of activation levels of stimulus representations does not underlie superior discrimination in autism. Cognition, 77, 81–96.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • O’Riordan, M. (2004). Superior visual search in adults with autism. Autism, 8(3), 229–248.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • O’Riordan, M., & Plaisted, K. (2001). Enhanced discrimination in autism. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 54(4), 961–979.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Oldfield, R. C. (1971). The assessment and analysis of handedness: The edinburgh inventory. Neuropsychologia, 9(1), 97–113.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Papadopoulos, N., McGinley, J., Tonge, B., Bradshaw, J., Saunders, K., Murphy, A., et al. (2012). Motor proficiency and emotional/behavioural disturbance in autism and Asperger’s disorder: Another piece of the neurological puzzle? Autism, 16(6), 627–640.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pellicano, E., Smith, A. D., Cristino, F., Hood, B. M., Briscoe, J., & Gilchrist, I. D. (2011). Children with autism are neither systematic nor optimal foragers. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 108(1), 421–426.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Plaisted, K., O’Riordan, M., & Baron-Cohen, S. (1998). Enhanced visual search for a conjunctive target in autism: A research note. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 39(5), 777–783.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ramnani, N. (2012). Frontal lobe and posterior parietal contributions to the cortico-cerebellar system. Cerebellum, 11(2), 366–383.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Remington, A., Swettenham, J., Campbell, R., & Coleman, M. (2009). Selective attention and perceptual load in autism spectrum disorder. Psychological Science, 20(11), 1388–1393.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Remington, A., Swettenham, J. G., & Lavie, N. (2012). Lightening the load: Perceptual load impairs visual detection in typical adults but not in autism. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 121(2), 544–551.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rinehart, N. J., Bellgrove, M. A., Tonge, B. J., Brereton, A. V., Howells-Rankin, D., & Bradshaw, J. L. (2006a). An examination of movement kinematics in young people with high-functioning autism and Asperger’s disorder: Further evidence for a motor planning deficit. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 36(6), 757–767.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rinehart, N. J., Bradshaw, J. L., Brereton, A. V., & Tonge, B. (2001a). Movement preparation in high-functioning autism and asperger disorder, a serial choice reaction time task involving motor reprogramming. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 31(1), 79–88.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rinehart, N. J., Bradshaw, J. L., Moss, S. A., Brereton, A. V., & Tonge, B. (2000). Atypical interference of local detail on global processing in high-functioning autism and Asperger’s disorder. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 41(6), 769–778.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rinehart, N. J., Bradshaw, J. L., Moss, S. A., Brereton, A. V., & Tonge, B. J. (2001b). A deficit in shifting attention present in high-functioning autism but not Asperger’s disorder. Autism, 5(1), 67–80.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rinehart, N. J., Bradshaw, J. L., Moss, S. A., Brereton, A. V., & Tonge, B. J. (2008). Brief report: Inhibition of return in young people with autism and Asperger’s disorder. Autism, 12(3), 249–260.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rinehart, N. J., Tonge, B., Bradshaw, J. L., Iansek, R., Enticott, P. G., & McGinley, J. (2006b). Gait function in high-functioning autism and Asperger’s disorder: Evidence for basal-ganglia and cerebellar involvement? European Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 15(5), 256–264.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rinehart, N. J., Tonge, B., Brereton, A., & Bradshaw, J. (2010). Attentional blink in young people with high-functioning autism and Asperger’s disorder. Autism, 14(1), 47–66.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Robertson, C. E., Kravitz, D. J., Freyberg, J., Baron-Cohen, S., & Baker, C. I. (2013). Tunnel vision: Sharper gradient of spatial attention in autism. The Journal of Neuroscience, 33(16), 6776–6781.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ronconi, L., Gori, S., Ruffino, M., Molteni, M., & Facoetti, A. (2013). Zoom-out attentional impairment in children with autism spectrum disorder. Cortex, 49(4), 1025–1033.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sattler, J. M., & Dumont, R. (2004). Assessment of children: WISC-IV and WPPSI-III supplement. San Diego: Sattler.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schmitz, C., & Rezaie, P. (2008). The neuropathology of autism: Where do we stand? Neuropathology and Applied Neurobiology, 34(1), 4–11.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Shirama, A., Kato, N., & Kashino, M. (2017). When do individuals with autism spectrum disorder show superiority in visual search? Autism, 21(8), 942–951.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sokolov, A. A., Miall, R. C., & Ivry, R. B. (2017). The cerebellum: Adaptive prediction for movement and cognition. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 21(5), 313–332.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stanley-Cary, C., Rinehart, N. J., Tonge, B., White, O., & Fielding, J. (2011). Greater disruption to control of voluntary saccades in autistic disorder than Asperger’s disorder: Evidence for greater cerebellar involvement in autism? Cerebellum, 10(1), 70–80.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stoodley, C. J. (2016). The cerebellum and neurodevelopmental disorders. Cerebellum, 15(1), 34–37.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tomlinson, S. P., Davis, N. J., & Bracewell, R. M. (2013). Brain stimulation studies of non-motor cerebellar function: A systematic review. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 37(5), 766–789.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Treisman, A. M., & Gelade, G. (1980). Feature integration theory of attention. Cognitive Psychology, 12, 97–136.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wang, S. S., Kloth, A. D., & Badura, A. (2014). The cerebellum, sensitive periods, and autism. Neuron, 83(3), 518–532.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Whitney, D., & Levi, D. M. (2011). Visual crowding: A fundamental limit on conscious perception and object recognition. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 15(4), 160–168.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wollmann, T., Barroso, J., Monton, F., & Nieto, A. (2002). Neuropsychological test performance of patients with Friedreich’s ataxia. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 24(5), 677–686.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

We would like to thank our participants and their families for kindly offering to participate in this study. We are also grateful to the paediatricians at Melbourne Children’s Clinic for assisting with recruitment. This paper has been prepared as part of a doctoral thesis.

Funding

The work was supported by doctoral research funding from the School of Psychological Sciences and Monash Institute of Cognitive and Clinical Neuroscience, Monash University, Clayton, VIC, 3800, Australia.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

EL and JF designed the study. NR advised on participant-related processes and assisted EL with recruitment. EL collected the data and performed data analysis. All authors were involved in manuscript drafting and approved the final manuscript.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Ebony Lindor.

Ethics declarations

Conflict of interest

NR has received funding from the Ferrero Group, Australia and Moose Toys. Ferrero Group, Australia and Moose Toys had no role in this research including the collection, analysis, and interpretation of data; in writing of the manuscript; and in the decision to submit the article for publication. NR has received speaker honorarium from Novartis (2002), Pfzier (2006) and Nutricia (2007). JF has also received research grants for Novartis (2015) and Sanofi-Genzyme (2017). NR is a Director of the Amaze Board (Autism Victoria). EL, NR and JF each declares that she has no conflict of interest.

Ethical Approval

All procedures performed in studies involving human participants were in accordance with the ethical standards of the institutional and/or national research committee and with the 1964 Helsinki declaration and its later amendments or comparable ethical standards.

Informed Consent

Informed consent was obtained from all individual participants included in the study.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Lindor, E., Rinehart, N. & Fielding, J. Superior Visual Search and Crowding Abilities Are Not Characteristic of All Individuals on the Autism Spectrum. J Autism Dev Disord 48, 3499–3512 (2018). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-018-3601-2

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-018-3601-2

Keywords

Navigation